Time-to-target explains task-dependent modulation of temporal feedback gain evolution

Visuomotor feedback gains vary in magnitude throughout a reach, commonly explained by optimal control. However, this pattern also matches the hand velocity profile, suggesting a simpler, perhaps hardwired, controller. Here we test between these alternatives, examining whether movement velocity regulates visuomotor gains. Feedback gain profiles were modulated across five conditions, with significant effects for altered hand, but not cursor, kinematics. Using optimal control we show that visuomotor feedback gains exhibit a consistent relation with time-to-target, rather than velocity, across a range of kinematics. Although our model suggests a time beyond which feedback gains should be down-regulated to minimise cost, our participants deliberately extend their movement durations for late perturbations, where feedback gains are reduced due to the lack of time for correction. Finally, we qualitatively replicate participant results within our model, but only when accounting for this extension of time-to-target, suggesting that expected time-to-target is critical in online feedback control.

[1]  J. Duysens,et al.  Postural responses to target jumps and background motion in a fast pointing task , 2018, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Internal models of target motion: expected dynamics overrides measured kinematics in timing manual interceptions. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  Daniel Bullock,et al.  Prospective control of manual interceptive actions: comparative simulations of extant and new model constructs , 2001, Neural Networks.

[4]  Lionel Rigoux,et al.  A Model of Reward- and Effort-Based Optimal Decision Making and Motor Control , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[5]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Persistence of motor adaptation during constrained, multi-joint, arm movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  Emanuel Todorov,et al.  Stochastic Optimal Control and Estimation Methods Adapted to the Noise Characteristics of the Sensorimotor System , 2005, Neural Computation.

[7]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Rapid motor responses are appropriately tuned to the metrics of a visuospatial task. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  D. Wolpert,et al.  The Temporal Evolution of Feedback Gains Rapidly Update to Task Demands , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Agnès Roby-Brami,et al.  Experimental and theoretical study of velocity fluctuations during slow movements in humans. , 2019, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Abigail A. Russo,et al.  Neural Trajectories in the Supplementary Motor Area and Motor Cortex Exhibit Distinct Geometries, Compatible with Different Classes of Computation , 2019, Neuron.

[11]  Brian L. Day,et al.  Direct visuomotor mapping for fast visually-evoked arm movements , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  L. Wasserman,et al.  Computing Bayes Factors by Combining Simulation and Asymptotic Approximations , 1997 .

[13]  Sae Franklin,et al.  Visuomotor feedback gains upregulate during the learning of novel dynamics , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Manu Chhabra,et al.  Flexible, Task-Dependent Use of Sensory Feedback to Control Hand Movements , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  E. Todorov,et al.  A generalized iterative LQG method for locally-optimal feedback control of constrained nonlinear stochastic systems , 2005, Proceedings of the 2005, American Control Conference, 2005..

[16]  Helen J. Huang,et al.  A Representation of Effort in Decision-Making and Motor Control , 2016, Current Biology.

[17]  Frédéric Crevecoeur,et al.  Rapid Online Selection between Multiple Motor Plans , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Motor Adaptation as a Process of Reoptimization , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  F. Horak,et al.  Postural feedback responses scale with biomechanical constraints in human standing , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  J. Vercher,et al.  Target and hand position information in the online control of goal-directed arm movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Sae Franklin,et al.  Temporal Evolution of Spatial Computations for Visuomotor Control , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  Stephen H Scott,et al.  Influence of the behavioral goal and environmental obstacles on rapid feedback responses. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  J Randall Flanagan,et al.  Parallel Specification of Visuomotor Feedback Gains during Bimanual Reaching to Independent Goals , 2017, eNeuro.

[24]  Kelvin E. Jones,et al.  The scaling of motor noise with muscle strength and motor unit number in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  E. Brenner,et al.  Fast and fine-tuned corrections when the target of a hand movement is displaced , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  Optimal Control Predicts Human Performance on Objects with Internal Degrees of Freedom , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[27]  Kelvin E. Jones,et al.  Sources of signal-dependent noise during isometric force production. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  A rapid visuomotor response on the human upper limb is selectively influenced by implicit motor learning. , 2019, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  J. Saunders,et al.  Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  Emanuel Todorov,et al.  Evidence for the Flexible Sensorimotor Strategies Predicted by Optimal Feedback Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Hubert Ripoll,et al.  Time-to-contact estimation of accelerated stimuli is based on first-order information. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  A modular planar robotic manipulandum with end-point torque control , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[33]  F Crevecoeur,et al.  Feedback responses rapidly scale with the urgency to correct for external perturbations. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  David W. Franklin,et al.  When Optimal Feedback Control Is Not Enough: Feedforward Strategies Are Required for Optimal Control with Active Sensing , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[35]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  Zhong-Ping Jiang,et al.  Movement Duration, Fitts's Law, and an Infinite-Horizon Optimal Feedback Control Model for Biological Motor Systems , 2013, Neural Computation.

[37]  David C Knill,et al.  Visual Feedback Control of Hand Movements , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[39]  B. Day,et al.  Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  J. Diedrichsen,et al.  A Dedicated Binding Mechanism for the Visual Control of Movement , 2014, Current Biology.

[41]  Sae Franklin,et al.  Rapid visuomotor feedback gains are tuned to the task dynamics , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Emmanuel Guigon,et al.  Computational Motor Control : Redundancy and Invariance , 2007 .

[43]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Optimal feedback control and the long-latency stretch response , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Specificity of Reflex Adaptation for Task-Relevant Variability , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[46]  David C. Knill,et al.  Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control both the direction and distance of pointing movements , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Sae Franklin,et al.  Fractionation of the visuomotor feedback response to directions of movement and perturbation , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Luigi Acerbi,et al.  Practical Bayesian Optimization for Model Fitting with Bayesian Adaptive Direct Search , 2017, NIPS.

[49]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  Etienne Olivier,et al.  Visual Responses on Neck Muscles Reveal Selective Gating that Prevents Express Saccades , 2004, Neuron.

[51]  J Randall Flanagan,et al.  Visuomotor feedback gains are modulated by gaze position. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  David W Franklin,et al.  Rapid Feedback Responses Arise From Precomputed Gains. , 2016, Motor control.

[53]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Does the brain model Newton's laws? , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  David W Franklin,et al.  Impedance control and internal model use during the initial stage of adaptation to novel dynamics in humans , 2005, The Journal of physiology.

[55]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  Emmanuel Guigon,et al.  Optimality, stochasticity, and variability in motor behavior , 2008, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.