Signal Reliability and Intraspecific Deception

Abstract A signal is considered to be reliable if (1) some feature of the signal is consistently correlated with an attribute of the signaler or its environment and (2) receivers benefit from knowing about that attribute. Signaling systems that do not provide reliable information may exist if signal features exploit a sensory bias of the receiver. At evolutionary equilibrium, however, signals are expected to be reliable on average, meaning that they are reliable enough that a receiver benefits overall from responding to them rather than ignoring them. When there is a conflict of interest between signaler and receiver, signal reliability requires some mechanism to be maintained. Proposed mechanisms include (1) physical and informational constraints on signal production; (2) signal costs that differentially affect signalers of varying quality (the handicap mechanism); (3) differential benefits, which produce honest signals of need; and (4) receiver-dependent costs, which produce conventional signals whose meaning is not related to their physical structure but rather results from an arbitrary convention. The requirement that signals be reliable only on average allows the possibility of some admixture of intra-specific deception, which has been observed in various types of signals, including signals used in aggression, courtship, predator warning, and begging.

[1]  E. Tibbetts,et al.  Visual signals of status and rival assessment in Polistes dominulus paper wasps , 2008, Biology Letters.

[2]  M. Enquist,et al.  A strategic taxonomy of biological communication , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[3]  R. Caldwell,et al.  Intraspecific Deception by Bluffing: A Defense Strategy of Newly Molted Stomatopods (Arthropoda: Crustacea) , 1983, Science.

[4]  K. L. Schmidt,et al.  Early-life stress affects song complexity, song learning and volume of the brain nucleus RA in adult male song sparrows , 2013, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  Thomas D. Seeley,et al.  Honey Bee Colonies are Group‐Level Adaptive Units , 1997, The American Naturalist.

[6]  M. Beekman,et al.  Honeybee waggle dance error: adaption or constraint? Unravelling the complex dance language of honeybees , 2014, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  T. Seeley,et al.  Dancing bees tune both duration and rate of waggle-run production in relation to nectar-source profitability , 2000, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[8]  N. Davies,et al.  Signals of need in parent–offspring communication and their exploitation by the common cuckoo , 1999, Nature.

[9]  R. Caldwell,et al.  Deceptive communication in asymmetric fights of the stomatopod crustacean Gonodactylus bredini , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[10]  R. D. Magrath,et al.  Calling at a cost: elevated nestling calling attracts predators to active nests , 2011, Biology Letters.

[11]  M. Ryan,et al.  Bat Predation and Sexual Advertisement in a Neotropical Anuran , 1982, The American Naturalist.

[12]  J. Mappes,et al.  Female choice for male drumming in the wolf spider Hygrolycosa rubrofasciata , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[13]  J. Dale,et al.  A socially enforced signal of quality in a paper wasp , 2004, Nature.

[14]  J. Mappes,et al.  SEXUAL SELECTION IN A WOLF SPIDER: MALE DRUMMING ACTIVITY, BODY SIZE, AND VIABILITY , 1996, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[15]  S. Peters,et al.  Effects of early nutrition on growth rate and adult size in song sparrows Melospiza melodia , 2004 .

[16]  T. Scott-Phillips Defining biological communication , 2008, Journal of evolutionary biology.

[17]  J. Kotiaho Testing the assumptions of conditional handicap theory: costs and condition dependence of a sexually selected trait , 2000, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[18]  T. Bilde,et al.  Evolution of deceit by worthless donations in a nuptial gift-giving spider , 2014 .

[19]  T. Clutton‐Brock,et al.  Signals of need in a cooperatively breeding mammal with mobile offspring , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[20]  P. Backwell,et al.  Dishonest signalling in a fiddler crab , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  J. Bro-Jørgensen,et al.  Male Topi Antelopes Alarm Snort Deceptively to Retain Females for Mating , 2010, The American Naturalist.

[22]  M. Mokkonen,et al.  The evolutionary ecology of deception , 2016, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[23]  P. Stoddard,et al.  Song-type matching in the song sparrow , 1992 .

[24]  N. Tamura Postcopulatory mate guarding by vocalization in the Formosan squirrel , 1995, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[25]  A. Zahavi Mate selection-a selection for a handicap. , 1975, Journal of theoretical biology.

[26]  James N. M. Smith,et al.  Hamilton and Zuk meet heterozygosity? Song repertoire size indicates inbreeding and immunity in song sparrows (Melospiza melodia) , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  William A. Searcy,et al.  Song repertoire size and female preferences in song sparrows , 1984, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[28]  P. Stoddard,et al.  Fitness Correlates of Song Repertoire Size in Free‐Living Song Sparrows (Melospiza melodia) , 2005, The American Naturalist.

[29]  S. Lorentsen,et al.  Begging and parent–offspring conflict in grey seals , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  Szabolcs Számadó,et al.  The cost of honesty and the fallacy of the handicap principle , 2011, Animal Behaviour.

[31]  J. Stynoski,et al.  Tadpole begging reveals high quality , 2017, Journal of evolutionary biology.

[32]  Brandon C. Wheeler Monkeys crying wolf? Tufted capuchin monkeys use anti-predator calls to usurp resources from conspecifics , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  N. Davies,et al.  Deep croaks and fighting assessment in toads Bufo bufo , 1978, Nature.

[34]  Johanna Mappes,et al.  Viability costs of condition-dependent sexual male display in a drumming wolf spider , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[35]  A. Grafen Biological signals as handicaps. , 1990, Journal of theoretical biology.

[36]  M. Ryan,et al.  THE SENSORY BASIS OF SEXUAL SELECTION FOR COMPLEX CALLS IN THE TÚNGARA FROG, PHYSALAEMUS PUSTULOSUS (SEXUAL SELECTION FOR SENSORY EXPLOITATION) , 1990, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[37]  M. Ryan,et al.  The Adaptive Significance of a Complex Vocal Repertoire in a Neotropical Frog , 2010 .

[38]  T. Guilford,et al.  What are conventional signals? , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[39]  K. Wells,et al.  Energetics of vocalization by an anuran amphibian (Hyla versicolor) , 1985, Journal of Comparative Physiology B.

[40]  S. Peters,et al.  Quality of song learning affects female response to male bird song , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[41]  A. Griffin,et al.  Cooperation and conflict in quorum-sensing bacterial populations , 2007, Nature.

[42]  Çağlar Akçay,et al.  Song type matching is an honest early threat signal in a hierarchical animal communication system , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[43]  T. Bilde,et al.  Worthless donations: male deception and female counter play in a nuptial gift-giving spider , 2011, BMC Evolutionary Biology.

[44]  W F Martin,et al.  Mechanics of sound production in toads of the genus Bufo: passive elements. , 1971, The Journal of experimental zoology.

[45]  J. E. Lloyd Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris: Firefly Femmes Fatales , 1965, Science.

[46]  R. Estes,et al.  Prey selection and hunting behavior of the African wild dog. , 1967 .

[47]  M. Enquist Communication during aggressive interactions with particular reference to variation in choice of behaviour , 1985, Animal Behaviour.

[48]  M. Ryan,et al.  The effect of signal complexity on localization performance in bats that localize frog calls , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[49]  R. Kilner A growth cost of begging in captive canary chicks , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[50]  S. Peters,et al.  Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the "nutritional stress hypothesis" , 2002, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[51]  H. Greene,et al.  Coral Snake Mimicry: Does It Occur? , 1981, Science.

[52]  Hanna Kokko,et al.  Evolutionarily stable strategies of age-dependent sexual advertisement , 1997, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[53]  S. A. Perrill,et al.  Responses To Conspecific Advertisement Calls in the Green Frog (Rana Clamitans) and Their Role in Male-Male Communication , 1996 .

[54]  A. Griffin,et al.  Sibling conflict and dishonest signaling in birds , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[55]  K. Yasukawa,et al.  Response to playback of nestling begging in the red-winged blackbird,Agelaius phoeniceus , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[56]  R. Dawkins,et al.  Animal signals: information or manipulation? , 1978 .