A Psychophysiological Examination of Cognitive Processing of and Affective Responses to Social Expectancy Violations

Several models of person perception predict that expectancy violations have both affective and cognitive consequences for the perceiver. Although extant evidence generally supports these claims, the temporal resolution of traditional self-report measures has limited researchers' ability to convincingly link underlying physiological processes with observed outcomes. In this study, we examined these issues by measuring brain (event-related brain potentials) and peripheral (facial electromyogram) electrophysiological activity while participants read positive and negative expectancy-consistent, expectancy-violating, expectancy-irrelevant, and semantically incongruent behavioral sentences about fictitious characters. The electromyogram results indicated that negative (but not positive) expectancy-violating behaviors elicited enhanced negative affect as early as 100 to 300 ms poststimulus. The event-related potentials showed enhanced positivities with latency exceeding 300 ms in response to expectancy violations and negative behaviors. Semantically incongruent sentence endings influenced a separate negative component (N400), suggesting fundamental differences between semantic- and behavior-consistency processing. This difference also was evident in participants' recall. Implications for theoretical models of expectancy violation are discussed.

[1]  W. V. Bingham,et al.  Expectancies , 1953 .

[2]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  If Attitudes Affect How Stimuli Are Processed, Should They Not Affect the Event-Related Brain Potential? , 1993 .

[3]  G. Zajicek,et al.  The Wisdom of the Body , 1934, Nature.

[4]  Nancy L. Stein,et al.  Psychological and Biological Approaches To Emotion , 1990 .

[5]  Karen E. Dill,et al.  Evaluations of Ingroup and Outgroup Members: The Role of Category-Based Expectancy Violation , 1997 .

[6]  M. Kutas,et al.  Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.

[7]  Michael A. Becker Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles , 1998 .

[8]  M. Millar,et al.  Processing time and the recall of inconsistent and consistent behaviors of individuals and groups. , 1984 .

[9]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[10]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[11]  E. Higgins,et al.  Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .

[12]  Tom Pyszczynski,et al.  Role of disconfirmed expectancies in the instigation of attributional processing. , 1981 .

[13]  C. Stangor,et al.  Memory for expectancy-congruent and expectancy-incongruent information: A review of the social and social developmental literatures. , 1992 .

[14]  U. Dimberg,et al.  Unconscious Facial Reactions to Emotional Facial Expressions , 2000, Psychological science.

[15]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Beyond Bipolar Conceptualizations and Measures: The Case of Attitudes and Evaluative Space , 1997, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[16]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Person memory and judgment. , 1989, Psychological review.

[17]  E. Donchin Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[18]  D. Hamilton,et al.  Cognitive organization of impressions: effects of incongruency in complex representations. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  M. Kutas,et al.  Event-related brain potentials during initial encoding and recognition memory of congruous and incongruous words , 1986 .

[20]  M. Kutas,et al.  Views on how the electrical activity that the brain generates reflects the functions of different language structures. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[21]  G. Peeters,et al.  Positive-Negative Asymmetry in Evaluations: The Distinction Between Affective and Informational Negativity Effects , 1990 .

[22]  Lee Osterhout,et al.  Brain potentials reflect violations of gender stereotypes , 1997, Memory & cognition.

[23]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.

[24]  B. Ann Bettencourt,et al.  Effects of Category-Based Expectancy Violation on Affect-Related Evaluations: Toward a Comprehensive Model , 2000 .

[25]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[26]  P. Ekman,et al.  Facial signs of emotional experience. , 1980 .

[27]  M. Kutas,et al.  An Event-Related Potential (ERP) Analysis of Semantic Congruity and Repetition Effects in Sentences , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  S. Fiske Social cognition and social perception. , 1993, Annual review of psychology.

[29]  N. Schwarz Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. , 1990 .

[30]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows function. , 1999 .

[31]  M. Coles Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[32]  R. Hastie Causes and effects of causal attribution , 1984 .

[33]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  Unobservable Facial Actions and Emotion , 1992 .

[34]  E Donchin,et al.  Effects of mnemonic strategy manipulation in a Von Restorff paradigm. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[35]  R. Buck,et al.  Communication of affect through facial expressions in humans. , 1972, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  V B CLINE,et al.  INTERPERSONAL PERCEPTION. , 1964, Progress in experimental personality research.

[37]  Linda A. Jackson,et al.  Stereotype effects of attributions, predictions, and evaluations: No two social judgments are quite alike. , 1993 .

[38]  J. Bargh,et al.  Individual construct accessibility, person memory, and the recall-judgment link: The case of information overload. , 1985 .

[39]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: the negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  A. J. Fridlund,et al.  Guidelines for human electromyographic research. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[41]  L. Jussim,et al.  The nature of stereotypes: a comparison and integration of three theories , 1987 .

[42]  M. Rugg,et al.  Electrophysiology of Mind: Event-Related Brain Potentials and Cognition , 1995 .

[43]  D. Hamilton,et al.  Perceiving persons and groups. , 1996, Psychological review.

[44]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  Attitudes to the Right: Evaluative Processing is Associated with Lateralized Late Positive Event-Related Brain Potentials , 1996 .

[45]  E. Higgins,et al.  Handbook of motivation and cognition : foundations of social behavior , 1991 .

[46]  J. Bargh Automaticity in social psychology. , 1996 .

[47]  N. Miller,et al.  The modern face of prejudice and structural features that moderate the effect of cooperation on affect. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.