Ontogeny of vocal signals in the little brown bat, Myotis lucifugus

This study reports developmental changes in the vocal signals produced by wild-caught pre-volant and volant Myotis lucifugusAudio recordings were made from young animals (1-33 days old) and adults (over 1 year of age). The animals were removed from an attic maternity roost and studied individually in a room below. To stimulate flight-related behaviours, animals were released from a launching platform via a hinged floor, and their vocalizations were recorded as they approached a soft foam pad below. When the hinged floor opened, the youngest animals (1-4 days old) typically dropped onto the pad, but older animals either flapped their wings to achieve some horizontal displacement (>4 days) or sustained horizontal flight (>17 days). Vocalizations recorded under these conditions showed frequency modulation characteristic of adult echolocation sounds, even in animals as young as 4 days. Ontogenetic trends showed an increase in sound frequency, an increase in sound repetition rate and a decrease in sound duration as the animals matured. These data are discussed in the context of the development of echolocation behaviour in bats.

[1]  Cynthia F. Moss,et al.  Behavioral Studies of Auditory Information Processing , 1995 .

[2]  R. Held,et al.  MOVEMENT-PRODUCED STIMULATION IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF VISUALLY GUIDED BEHAVIOR. , 1963, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[3]  M. Fenton,et al.  The role of infant isolation calls in mother–infant reunions in the little brown bat, Myotis lucifugus (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae) , 1985 .

[4]  G. McCracken,et al.  Individual variation in the isolation calls of Mexican free-tailed bat pups (Tadarida brasiliensis mexicana) , 1986, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  E. Gould Experimental studies of the ontogeny of ultrasonic vocalizations in bats. , 1975, Developmental psychobiology.

[6]  J A Simmons,et al.  Physiological mechanisms for spatial filtering and image enhancement in the sonar of bats. , 1984, Annual review of physiology.

[7]  S. Matsumura,et al.  Mother-Infant Communication in a Horseshoe Bat (Rhinolophus ferrumequinum nippon): Development of Vocalization , 1979 .

[8]  松村 澄子 Mother-infant communication in a horseshoe bat (Rhinolophus ferrumequinum nippon) , 1980 .

[9]  Gareth Jones,et al.  The development of vocalizations in Pipistrellus pipistrellus (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae) during post-natal growth and the maintenance of individual vocal signatures , 1991 .

[10]  E. P. Animal Behaviour , 1901, Nature.

[11]  R. Suthers,et al.  Mechanisms of Sound Production by Echolocating Bats , 1973 .

[12]  Hans-Ulrich Schnitzler,et al.  Performance of Airborne Animal Sonar Systems: I. Microchiroptera , 1980 .

[13]  R. Busnel,et al.  Animal Sonar Systems , 1980, NATO Advanced Study Institutes Series.

[14]  T. Kunz,et al.  Age Estimation and Post-Natal Growth in the Bat Myotis lucifugus , 1982 .

[15]  A Hein,et al.  Eye movements initiate visual-motor development in the cat. , 1979, Science.

[16]  F. A. Webster,et al.  The Determination of Distance by Echolocating Bats , 1964, Nature.

[17]  A. Grinnell,et al.  Echolocation Ontogeny in Bats , 1980 .

[18]  E. Gould,et al.  Double-note communication calls in bats: occurrence in three families. , 1973, Journal of mammalogy.

[19]  D. Griffin,et al.  Laryngeal mechanisms in bats for the production of orientation sounds. , 1961, The Journal of experimental zoology.

[20]  D. Griffin,et al.  Listening in the Dark , 1959 .