Frontal-striatal dysfunction during planning in obsessive-compulsive disorder.

BACKGROUND Dysfunction of frontal-striatal, particularly orbitofrontal-striatal, circuitry has been implicated in the pathophysiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), characterized by obsessions, ritualistic behavior, anxiety, and specific cognitive impairments. In addition, neuropsychological studies in OCD have reported impairments in visuospatial tasks and executive functions, such as planning. OBJECTIVE To determine whether dorsal prefrontal-striatal dysfunction mediates planning impairment in patients with OCD. DESIGN A parametric self-paced pseudorandomized event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging version of the Tower of London task was used in 22 medication-free patients with OCD and 22 healthy control subjects. This paradigm, allowing flexible responding and post hoc classification of correct responses, was developed to compare groups likely to differ in performance. RESULTS Behavioral results showed significant planning impairments in OCD patients compared with control subjects. During planning, decreased frontal-striatal responsiveness was found in OCD patients, mainly in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and caudate nucleus. In addition, OCD patients showed increased, presumably compensatory, involvement of brain areas known to play a role in performance monitoring and short-term memory processing, such as anterior cingulate, ventrolateral prefrontal, and parahippocampal cortices. CONCLUSIONS These findings support the hypothesis that decreased dorsal prefrontal-striatal responsiveness is associated with impaired planning capacity in OCD patients. Because the described frontal-striatal dysfunction in OCD is independent of state anxiety and disease symptom severity, we conclude that executive impairment is a core feature in OCD.

[1]  Lull Gf,et al.  THE AMERICAN MEDICAL ASSOCIATION. , 1947, Science.

[2]  H A Simon,et al.  The theory of learning by doing. , 1979, Psychological review.

[3]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[4]  D. Cohen,et al.  Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Evidence supporting a genetic relationship. , 1986, Archives of general psychiatry.

[5]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Local cerebral glucose metabolic rates in obsessive-compulsive disorder. A comparison with rates in unipolar depression and in normal controls. , 1987, Archives of general psychiatry.

[7]  E. Sanavio Obsessions and compulsions: the Padua Inventory. , 1988, Behaviour research and therapy.

[8]  J. Rapoport,et al.  Cerebral glucose metabolism in childhood-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1989, Archives of general psychiatry.

[9]  T. Nordahl,et al.  Cerebral glucose metabolic rates in obsessive compulsive disorder. , 1989, Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology.

[10]  W. Goodman,et al.  The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale. I. Development, use, and reliability. , 1989, Archives of general psychiatry.

[11]  M. Taylor,et al.  Neuropsychological deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder , 1991 .

[12]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Caudate glucose metabolic rate changes with both drug and behavior therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1992, Archives of general psychiatry.

[13]  B. Toone,et al.  Neural correlates of planning ability: Frontal lobe activation during the tower of London test , 1993, Schizophrenia Research.

[14]  N C Andreasen,et al.  The neuropsychology of the prefrontal cortex. , 1993, Archives of neurology.

[15]  R. Morris,et al.  Neural correlates of planning ability: Frontal lobe activation during the tower of London test , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Baxter Lr Positron emission tomography studies of cerebral glucose metabolism in obsessive compulsive disorder. , 1994 .

[17]  Michael Petrides,et al.  Frontal lobes and behaviour , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[18]  C D Frith,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Obsessive–Compulsive Phenomena , 1994, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[19]  N. Alpert,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow measured during symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder using oxygen 15-labeled carbon dioxide and positron emission tomography. , 1994, Archives of general psychiatry.

[20]  E. Garcia-Rill,et al.  The pedunculopontine nucleus—Auditory input, arousal and pathophysiology , 1995, Progress in Neurobiology.

[21]  P. Emmelkamp,et al.  The structure of obsessive-compulsive symptoms. , 1995, Behaviour research and therapy.

[22]  G. Aston-Jones,et al.  Role of the locus coeruleus in emotional activation. , 1996, Progress in brain research.

[23]  M. Deiber,et al.  A controlled positron emission tomography study of obsessive and neutral auditory stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder with checking rituals , 1996, Psychiatry Research.

[24]  J. R. Baker,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1996, Archives of general psychiatry.

[25]  R. Dolan,et al.  Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  A M Owen,et al.  Specific cognitive deficits in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in obsessive–compulsive disorder , 1996, Psychological Medicine.

[27]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Planning and Spatial Working Memory: a Positron Emission Tomography Study in Humans , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[28]  M. Petrides,et al.  Specialized systems for the processing of mnemonic information within the primate frontal cortex. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[29]  N. Alpert,et al.  Probing striatal function in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a PET study of implicit sequence learning. , 1997, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[30]  E Garcia-Rill,et al.  Disorders of the reticular activating system. , 1997, Medical hypotheses.

[31]  C. Frith,et al.  Prefrontal dysfunction in depressed patients performing a complex planning task: a study using positron emission tomography , 1997, Psychological Medicine.

[32]  S Dehaene,et al.  A hierarchical neuronal network for planning behavior. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: Implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[34]  P. Maruff,et al.  Neuropsychological deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison with unipolar depression, panic disorder, and normal controls. , 1998, Archives of general psychiatry.

[35]  A L Brody,et al.  Neuroimaging and frontal-subcortical circuitry in obsessive-compulsive disorder , 1998, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[36]  Paul Maruff,et al.  Cognitive Deficits in Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder on Tests of Frontal–Striatal Function , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[37]  F. Hohagen,et al.  Cognitive Frontal Lobe Dysfunction in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[38]  C. Junqué,et al.  Neuropsychological functioning in a subclinical obsessive-compulsive sample , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[39]  A. Dagher,et al.  Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Role of locus coeruleus in attention and behavioral flexibility , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[41]  J. Cohen,et al.  The role of locus coeruleus in the regulation of cognitive performance. , 1999, Science.

[42]  S. Holland,et al.  fMRI of neuronal activation with symptom provocation in unmedicated patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. , 2000, Journal of psychiatric research.

[43]  E. Garcia-Rill,et al.  Locus coeruleus involvement in the effects of immobilization stress on the P13 midlatency auditory evoked potential in the rat , 2000, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[44]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Segregation of working memory functions within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  F Barkhof,et al.  Visualizing brain activation during planning: the tower of London test adapted for functional MR imaging. , 2000, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[46]  T. Shallice,et al.  The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  E. Fliers,et al.  Interaction of prefrontal cortical and hypothalamic systems in the pathogenesis of depression. , 2000, Progress in brain research.

[48]  G. Goodwin,et al.  Cognitive deficits in depression: Possible implications for functional neuropathology , 2001, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[49]  Sung-Cheng Huang,et al.  Cerebral metabolism in major depression and obsessive-compulsive disorder occurring separately and concurrently , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[50]  D. V. Cramon,et al.  Subprocesses of Performance Monitoring: A Dissociation of Error Processing and Response Competition Revealed by Event-Related fMRI and ERPs , 2001, NeuroImage.

[51]  S. Rauch,et al.  Probing striato-thalamic function in obsessive-compulsive disorder and Tourette syndrome using neuroimaging methods. , 2001, Advances in neurology.

[52]  R. E. Passingham,et al.  Imaging the mental components of a planning task , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  Bertrand Gaymard,et al.  Cortical control of spatial memory in humans: The visuooculomotor model , 2002, Annals of neurology.

[54]  W Cahn,et al.  Excessive recruitment of neural systems subserving logical reasoning in schizophrenia. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[55]  R. Simons,et al.  Error-related brain activity in obsessive–compulsive undergraduates , 2002, Psychiatry Research.

[56]  D. Amaral,et al.  Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: Projections to the neocortex , 2002, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[57]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Prefrontal cortex involvement in processing incorrect arithmetic equations: Evidence from event‐related fMRI , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[58]  W. K. Berg,et al.  The Tower of London Spatial Problem-Solving Task: Enhancing Clinical and Research Implementation , 2002, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[59]  S. Rauch,et al.  A study of parallel implicit and explicit information processing in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 2002, The American journal of psychiatry.

[60]  B. Richmond,et al.  Anterior Cingulate: Single Neuronal Signals Related to Degree of Reward Expectancy , 2002, Science.

[61]  John R Anderson,et al.  Neural mechanisms of planning: A computational analysis using event-related fMRI , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[62]  H. Garavan,et al.  Dissociable Executive Functions in the Dynamic Control of Behavior: Inhibition, Error Detection, and Correction , 2002, NeuroImage.

[63]  C. Carter,et al.  The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.

[64]  D. McKay,et al.  Neuropsychology of obsessive-compulsive disorder: a review and treatment implications. , 2003, Clinical psychology review.

[65]  Bill Faw Pre-frontal executive committee for perception, working memory, attention, long-term memory, motor control, and thinking: A tutorial review , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.

[66]  E. Miller,et al.  Neural circuits subserving the retrieval and maintenance of abstract rules. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[67]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Overactive Action Monitoring in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder , 2003, Psychological science.

[68]  Raymond J Dolan,et al.  Maintenance versus manipulation in verbal working memory revisited: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[69]  Sharlene D. Newman,et al.  Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  Frederik Barkhof,et al.  Frontostriatal system in planning complexity: a parametric functional magnetic resonance version of tower of london task , 2003, NeuroImage.

[71]  Steven C. R. Williams,et al.  Neural correlates of anxiety associated with obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions in normal volunteers , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[72]  Ulrich Schall,et al.  Functional brain maps of Tower of London performance: a positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[73]  Nick F. Ramsey,et al.  Spatial working memory deficits in obsessive compulsive disorder are associated with excessive engagement of the medial frontal cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[74]  M. H Beauchamp,et al.  Dynamic functional changes associated with cognitive skill learning of an adapted version of the Tower of London task , 2003, NeuroImage.

[75]  R Valabrègue,et al.  Individual differences in prefrontal cortical activation on the Tower of London planning task: implication for effortful processing , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[76]  M. Phillips,et al.  Distinct neural correlates of washing, checking, and hoarding symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 2004, Archives of general psychiatry.

[77]  Stephan Heckers,et al.  Hemispheric specialization of the lateral prefrontal cortex for strategic processing during spatial and shape working memory , 2004, NeuroImage.