Regretted Messages

This study investigates the cognitive processes that precede and follow the saying of a regretted message. Participants recalled and described real-life regretted messages. Three cognitive antecedents to saying such messages were investigated: competing demands on capacity, the situated importance of secondary goals, and goals realized by the messages. Six reasons for reflecting on regretted messages were measured. Results showed the most frequently reported competing demands on capacity to be stress, frustration, anger, and having a lot on one’s mind. The goals realized most often by regretted messages were expressing one’s true feelings, providing information, correcting a hearer’s behavior, and warning/threatening a hearer. For messages conflicting with impression and relationship maintenance goals, the situated importance of the goals positively predicted the seriousness of the goal conflict. Reflection on regretted messages focused most frequently on repairing the relationship, formulating a rule to avoid a recurrence, and reducing dissonance.

[1]  Steven R. Wilson,et al.  Identity Implications of Influence Goals , 2000 .

[2]  Carlos G. Aleman,et al.  Identity Implications of Influence Goals: A Revised Analysis of Face-Threatening Acts and Application to Seeking Compliance with Same-Sex Friends. , 1998 .

[3]  Walid A. Afifi,et al.  Characteristics and Consequences of Expectation Violations in Close Relationships , 1998 .

[4]  Janet R. Meyer Effect of secondary goal importance on the anticipation of message outcomes , 2005 .

[5]  Steven R. Wilson Development and test of a cognitive rules model of interaction goals , 1990 .

[6]  H. Dan O'Hair,et al.  The Management of Failure Events: Some Contextual Determinants of Accounting Behavior , 1983 .

[7]  Janet R. Meyer Pursuing relational goals in requests: The effects of dispositional factors and type of relationship , 2001 .

[8]  Janet R. Meyer Formulating plans for requests: An investigation of retrieval processes , 1994 .

[9]  James Price Dillard,et al.  Primary and secondary goals in the production of interpersonal influence messages , 1989 .

[10]  Steven A. Mccornack Information manipulation theory , 1992 .

[11]  John H. Harvey,et al.  Attribution, communication behavior, and close relationships , 2001 .

[12]  Margaret McLaughlin,et al.  The psychology of tactical communication. , 1990 .

[13]  S. Metts,et al.  Situational influence on the use of remedial strategies in embarrassing predicaments , 1989 .

[14]  K. Witte Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model , 1992 .

[15]  Effect of Primary Goal on Secondary Goal Importance and Message Plan Acceptability , 2009 .

[16]  Charles R. Berger,et al.  When a Lot of Knowledge Is a Dangerous Thing: The Debilitating Effects of Plan Complexity on Verbal Fluency. , 1989 .

[17]  John O. Greene,et al.  Complexity Effects on Temporal Characteristics of Speech , 1995 .

[18]  Janet R. Meyer Contextual influences on the pursuit of secondary goals in request messages , 2002 .

[19]  Min-Sun Kim,et al.  Cross-cultural comparisons of the perceived importance of conversational constraints , 1994 .

[20]  Janet R. Meyer Effect of verbal aggressiveness on the perceived importance of secondary goals in messages , 2004 .

[21]  Jennifer A. Samp,et al.  Communicative Responses to Problematic Events in Close Relationships I , 1998 .

[22]  John A. Daly,et al.  Regrettable messages: things people wish they Hadn't said , 1986 .

[23]  L. Festinger,et al.  A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance , 2017 .

[24]  Dale Hample Arguing: Exchanging Reasons Face to Face , 2012 .

[25]  Judith M. Dallinger,et al.  Individual Differences in Cognitive Editing Standards , 1987 .