Reproducibility in experimentation – the implications for regulatory toxicology

A paper based on the Sir William Paton lecture of the British Society of Toxicology, 2014. Sir William Paton's classical exploitation of nitrogen in mixtures of gases for deep-sea diving is a good example of what would now be called “blue skies” research where the data obtained were used as the basis for subsequent hypothesis-driven work. Like his work on acetyl choline, the original observations were the basis for further painstaking and step-like progression by experimentation, where each I was dotted and T crossed as the establishment of a therapy or practice was built into a clinical framework or workplace. However, in recent times there has been a failure to follow a similar cautious and pragmatic practice of advancement of knowledge at the interface of Science and Policy. Inadequately investigated associations are made the basis of speculative therapeutic or dietary interventions in frameworks where lack of effect is often not demonstrable and where the possibility of harm is seldom considered, notably in the dietary field. In the same way, isolated findings in regulatory studies may lead to poor decision making or to extensive work to explain non-significant events. The Economist has pointed out that venture capitalists use the rule that only 50% of academic study results can be replicated and Begley and Ellis report that a study of 53 “landmark” papers in Oncology found that only 6 were reproducible even with co-operation of the original authors. So what notice should be taken of single adverse toxicological studies in animals? Of isolated, epidemiologically determined associations? What is the scientific value of an individual regulatory study? What is the probability of error? What is the role of chance and variation? Only an understanding of mechanisms of production of the effect in question can resolve these issues.

[1]  S. Lazic,et al.  A call for transparent reporting to optimize the predictive value of preclinical research , 2012, Nature.

[2]  C. Berry Relativism, regulation and the dangers of indifferent science: the Sir Roy Cameron lecture of the Royal College of Pathologists. , 2010, Toxicology.

[3]  Malcolm Macleod,et al.  How can we improve the pre-clinical development of drugs for stroke? , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.

[4]  M. Beck,et al.  Pharmacokinetic, neurochemical, stereological and neuropathological studies on the potential effects of paraquat in the substantia nigra pars compacta and striatum of male C57BL/6J mice. , 2013, Neurotoxicology.

[5]  F. Prinz,et al.  Believe it or not: how much can we rely on published data on potential drug targets? , 2011, Nature Reviews Drug Discovery.

[6]  Fruits and vegetables and renal cell carcinoma: Findings from the European prospective investigation into cancer and nutrition (EPIC) , 2006 .

[7]  J. Ioannidis Why Most Published Research Findings Are False , 2019, CHANCE.

[8]  M. Beck,et al.  Dietary administration of paraquat for 13 weeks does not result in a loss of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra of C57BL/6J mice. , 2014, Regulatory toxicology and pharmacology : RTP.

[9]  S. Cohen Evaluation of Possible Carcinogenic Risk to Humans Based on Liver Tumors in Rodent Assays , 2010, Toxicologic pathology.

[10]  I. Shaw,et al.  Bioactive isoflavones in functional foods: the importance of gut microflora on bioavailability. , 2003, Nutrition reviews.

[11]  Michael Benatar,et al.  Methodological Quality of Animal Studies of Neuroprotective Agents Currently in Phase II/III Acute Ischemic Stroke Trials , 2009, Stroke.

[12]  P. Christou,et al.  Plurality of opinion, scientific discourse and pseudoscience: an in depth analysis of the Séralini et al. study claiming that Roundup™ Ready corn or the herbicide Roundup™ cause cancer in rats , 2013, Transgenic Research.

[13]  Samuel M Cohen,et al.  Human carcinogenic risk evaluation: an alternative approach to the two-year rodent bioassay. , 2004, Toxicological sciences : an official journal of the Society of Toxicology.

[14]  Paolo Vineis,et al.  Consumption of vegetables and fruits and risk of breast cancer. , 2005, JAMA.

[15]  J. Manson,et al.  Vitamins E and C in the prevention of prostate and total cancer in men: the Physicians' Health Study II randomized controlled trial. , 2009, JAMA.

[16]  U. Nöthlings,et al.  Fruit and vegetable intake and overall cancer risk in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC). , 2011, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[17]  J. Manson,et al.  Vitamin E in the Primary Prevention of Cardiovascular Disease and Cancer: The Women’s Health Study: A Randomized Controlled Trial , 2005, JAMA.

[18]  Benjamin Haibe-Kains,et al.  Inconsistency in large pharmacogenomic studies , 2013, Nature.

[19]  M. Bracken Why are so many epidemiology associations inflated or wrong? Does poorly conducted animal research suggest implausible hypotheses? , 2009, Annals of epidemiology.

[20]  D. Fanelli Why Growing Retractions Are (Mostly) a Good Sign , 2013, PLoS medicine.

[21]  J. Manson,et al.  Vitamins E and C in the prevention of cardiovascular disease in men: the Physicians' Health Study II randomized controlled trial. , 2008, JAMA.

[22]  David Moher,et al.  Development of AMSTAR: a measurement tool to assess the methodological quality of systematic reviews , 2007, BMC medical research methodology.

[23]  S. Yusuf,et al.  Effects of long-term vitamin E supplementation on cardiovascular events and cancer: a randomized controlled trial. , 2005, JAMA.

[24]  J. Reis-Filho,et al.  Pathology in drug discovery and development , 2014, The Journal of pathology.

[25]  C. Begley,et al.  Drug development: Raise standards for preclinical cancer research , 2012, Nature.

[26]  K. Dickersin How important is publication bias? A synthesis of available data. , 1997, AIDS education and prevention : official publication of the International Society for AIDS Education.

[27]  John P A Ioannidis,et al.  Empirical evaluation of very large treatment effects of medical interventions. , 2012, JAMA.

[28]  I. Boyd Research: A standard for policy-relevant science , 2013, Nature.

[29]  W. Sutherland Review by quality not quantity for better policy , 2013, Nature.

[30]  Jeremy Grimshaw,et al.  AMSTAR is a reliable and valid measurement tool to assess the methodological quality of systematic reviews. , 2009, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[31]  Elio Riboli,et al.  Epidemiologic evidence of the protective effect of fruit and vegetables on cancer risk. , 2003, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[32]  E. Larsson,et al.  Antioxidants Accelerate Lung Cancer Progression in Mice , 2014, Science Translational Medicine.