Functional connectivity of the cortical network supporting statistical learning in musicians and non-musicians: an MEG study

Statistical learning is a cognitive process of great importance for the detection and representation of environmental regularities. Complex cognitive processes such as statistical learning usually emerge as a result of the activation of widespread cortical areas functioning in dynamic networks. The present study investigated the cortical large-scale network supporting statistical learning of tone sequences in humans. The reorganization of this network related to musical expertise was assessed via a cross-sectional comparison of a group of musicians to a group of non-musicians. The cortical responses to a statistical learning paradigm incorporating an oddball approach were measured via Magnetoencephalographic (MEG) recordings. Large-scale connectivity of the cortical activity was calculated via a statistical comparison of the estimated transfer entropy in the sources’ activity. Results revealed the functional architecture of the network supporting the processing of statistical learning, highlighting the prominent role of informational processing pathways that bilaterally connect superior temporal and intraparietal sources with the left IFG. Musical expertise is related to extensive reorganization of this network, as the group of musicians showed a network comprising of more widespread and distributed cortical areas as well as enhanced global efficiency and increased contribution of additional temporal and frontal sources in the information processing pathway.

[1]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  Network-based statistic: Identifying differences in brain networks , 2010, NeuroImage.

[2]  Olaf Sporns,et al.  Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations , 2010, NeuroImage.

[3]  K. Okanoya,et al.  Statistical segmentation of tone sequences activates the left inferior frontal cortex: A near-infrared spectroscopy study , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  E. Newport,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article INCIDENTAL LANGUAGE LEARNING: Ustening (and Learning) out of the Comer of Your Ear , 2022 .

[5]  O. Sporns,et al.  Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[6]  Roy D. Patterson,et al.  Predictive Coding and Pitch Processing in the Auditory Cortex , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  N. Kraus,et al.  The auditory brainstem is a barometer of rapid auditory learning , 2013, Neuroscience.

[8]  D. Schön,et al.  Music training for the development of speech segmentation. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  M. Tervaniemi,et al.  Pitch discrimination accuracy in musicians vs nonmusicians: an event-related potential and behavioral study , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  Josep Marco-Pallarés,et al.  Combined ICA-LORETA analysis of mismatch negativity , 2005, NeuroImage.

[11]  Daniele Schön,et al.  Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr022 Musical Expertise Boosts Implicit Learning of Both Musical and Linguistic Structures , 2011 .

[12]  Scott Sinnett,et al.  Speech segmentation by statistical learning depends on attention , 2005, Cognition.

[13]  Sibylle C. Herholz,et al.  Looking for a pattern: An MEG study on the abstract mismatch negativity in musicians and nonmusicians , 2009, BMC Neuroscience.

[14]  Francisco del Pozo,et al.  HERMES: Towards an Integrated Toolbox to Characterize Functional and Effective Brain Connectivity , 2013, Neuroinformatics.

[15]  A. Roepstorff,et al.  Predictive coding of music – Brain responses to rhythmic incongruity , 2009, Cortex.

[16]  Daniele Schön,et al.  Learning of musical and linguistic structures: comparing event-related potentials and behavior , 2010, Neuroreport.

[17]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Lateralization of ventral and dorsal auditory-language pathways in the human brain , 2005, NeuroImage.

[18]  Antoni Rodríguez-Fornells,et al.  Brain Dynamics Sustaining Rapid Rule Extraction from Speech , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Ronny K. Ibrahim,et al.  Statistical learning and auditory processing in children with music training: An ERP study , 2017, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[20]  M. Yumoto,et al.  Implicit and explicit statistical learning of tone sequences across spectral shifts , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  Thomas R. Knösche,et al.  A guideline for head volume conductor modeling in EEG and MEG , 2014, NeuroImage.

[22]  Gordon Pipa,et al.  Transfer entropy—a model-free measure of effective connectivity for the neurosciences , 2010, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[23]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cracking the Language Code: Neural Mechanisms Underlying Speech Parsing , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  S. Durrant,et al.  Commentary: Musicians' Online Performance during Auditory and Visual Statistical Learning Tasks , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[25]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Functional segregation within pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus: evidence from fMRI studies of imitation and action observation. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  B. Tillmann,et al.  Cognitive and methodological considerations on the effects of musical expertise on speech segmentation , 2012, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[27]  L. Craighero,et al.  Broca's Area in Language, Action, and Music , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[28]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[29]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A theory of cortical responses , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[30]  H. Aarts,et al.  Learning of predictive relations between events depends on attention, not on awareness , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[31]  A. Friederici The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  The Plasticity of the Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus as a Function of Musical Expertise: A Diffusion Tensor Imaging Study , 2009, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[33]  Sibylle C. Herholz,et al.  Statistical learning effects in musicians and non-musicians: An MEG study , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  M. Yumoto,et al.  Pitch-class distribution modulates the statistical learning of atonal chord sequences , 2016, Brain and Cognition.

[35]  Panagiotis D Bamidis,et al.  Musical expertise is related to altered functional connectivity during audiovisual integration , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  M. Tettamanti,et al.  Broca's Area: a Supramodal Hierarchical Processor? , 2006, Cortex.

[37]  S. Koelsch,et al.  Effects of musical expertise on the early right anterior negativity: an event-related brain potential study. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[38]  A. Friederici,et al.  Musical syntax is processed in Broca's area: an MEG study , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  Patrick Berg,et al.  Artifact Correction of the Ongoing EEG Using Spatial Filters Based on Artifact and Brain Signal Topographies , 2002, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[40]  Barbara Landau,et al.  The Necessity of the Medial Temporal Lobe for Statistical Learning , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Sibylle C. Herholz,et al.  Evidence for Training-Induced Plasticity in Multisensory Brain Structures: An MEG Study , 2012, PloS one.

[42]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Implicit Perceptual Anticipation Triggered by Statistical Learning , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  Salomi S. Asaridou,et al.  Repetition Suppression in the Left Inferior Frontal Gyrus Predicts Tone Learning Performance. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  P. Hibbard,et al.  Summation of visual attributes in auditory–visual crossmodal correspondences , 2017, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[46]  Philippe Peigneux,et al.  Auditory Magnetoencephalographic Frequency-Tagged Responses Mirror the Ongoing Segmentation Processes Underlying Statistical Learning , 2016, Brain Topography.

[47]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Broca's Area and the Hierarchical Organization of Human Behavior , 2006, Neuron.

[48]  Stefan Koelsch,et al.  Under the hood of statistical learning: A statistical MMN reflects the magnitude of transitional probabilities in auditory sequences , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[49]  K. Flowers,et al.  Chance guessing in a forced-choice recognition task and the detection of malingering. , 2008, Neuropsychology.

[50]  J. Staiger,et al.  Increased corpus callosum size in musicians , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: a new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[52]  A. Rodríguez-Fornells,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms involved in language learning in adults , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[53]  Joanne Arciuli,et al.  Musicians’ Online Performance during Auditory and Visual Statistical Learning Tasks , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[54]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Domain generality versus modality specificity: the paradox of statistical learning , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  Hierarchical artificial grammar processing engages Broca's area , 2008, NeuroImage.

[56]  Felix Darvas,et al.  Spatio-temporal source imaging reveals subcomponents of the human auditory mismatch negativity in the cingulum and right inferior temporal gyrus , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.

[57]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Where and When the Anterior Cingulate Cortex Modulates Attentional Response: Combined fMRI and ERP Evidence , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[58]  Richard N. Aslin,et al.  The neural correlates of statistical learning in a word segmentation task: An fMRI study , 2013, Brain and Language.

[59]  Max C. Keuken,et al.  TMS over M1 Reveals Expression and Selective Suppression of Conflicting Action Impulses , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[60]  Daniele Schön,et al.  Musical Expertise and Statistical Learning of Musical and Linguistic Structures , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[61]  John Shawe-Taylor,et al.  Neural prediction of higher-order auditory sequence statistics , 2011, NeuroImage.

[62]  J. Winer Decoding the auditory corticofugal systems , 2005, Hearing Research.

[63]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults , 1999, Cognition.

[64]  C. Gilbert,et al.  The Neural Basis of Perceptual Learning , 2001, Neuron.

[65]  Kazuo Okanoya,et al.  On-line Assessment of Statistical Learning by Event-related Potentials , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[66]  Christo Pantev,et al.  Plasticity of the human auditory cortex related to musical training , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[67]  Anatol C. Kreitzer,et al.  Plasticity in gray and white: neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[68]  E. Newport,et al.  WORD SEGMENTATION : THE ROLE OF DISTRIBUTIONAL CUES , 1996 .

[69]  Emily B. J. Coffey,et al.  Task decomposition: a framework for comparing diverse training models in human brain plasticity studies , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[70]  Yong He,et al.  BrainNet Viewer: A Network Visualization Tool for Human Brain Connectomics , 2013, PloS one.

[71]  D. Schön,et al.  Neural sensitivity to statistical regularities as a fundamental biological process that underlies auditory learning: The role of musical practice , 2014, Hearing Research.

[72]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[73]  Julia Uddén,et al.  A rostro-caudal gradient of structured sequence processing in the left inferior frontal gyrus , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.