Young children overimitate in third-party contexts.

The exhibition of actions that are causally unnecessary to the outcomes with which they are associated is a core feature of human cultural behavior. To enter into the world(s) of their cultural in-group, children must learn to assimilate such unnecessary actions into their own behavioral repertoire. Past research has established the habitual tendency of children to adopt the redundant actions of adults demonstrated directly to them. Here we document how young children will do so even when such actions are modeled to a third person regardless of whether children are presented with the test apparatus by the demonstrating, and assumedly expert, adult or by the observing, and assumedly naive, adult (Experiment 1), whether or not children had opportunity to discover how the apparatus works prior to modeling (Experiment 1), and whether or not children's attention was drawn to the demonstration while they were otherwise occupied (Experiment 2). These results emphasize human children's readiness to acquire behavior that is in keeping with what others do, regardless of the apparent efficiency of the actions employed, and in so doing to participate in cultural learning.

[1]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[2]  J. Searle The Construction of Social Reality , 1997 .

[3]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Push or Pull: Imitation vs. Emulation in Great Apes and Human Children , 2006 .

[4]  N. McGuigan,et al.  Cultural transmission of irrelevant tool actions in diffusion chains of 3- and 5-year-old children , 2010 .

[5]  M. Nielsen,et al.  Copying actions and copying outcomes: social learning through the second year. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[6]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery , 2011, Cognition.

[7]  A. Whiten,et al.  From over-imitation to super-copying: adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. , 2011, British journal of psychology.

[8]  Nameera Akhtar,et al.  The robustness of learning through overhearing. , 2005, Developmental science.

[9]  Mark Nielsen,et al.  Over‐imitation in children with autism and Down syndrome , 2010 .

[10]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). , 1993 .

[11]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[12]  Mark Nielsen,et al.  Imitation, pretend play, and childhood: essential elements in the evolution of human culture? , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.

[13]  G. Simcock,et al.  The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds' imitation from live and televised models. , 2008, Developmental science.

[14]  Moritz M. Daum,et al.  Fourteen-month-olds' imitation of differently aged models , 2012 .

[15]  Emma Flynn,et al.  Investigating children as cultural magnets: do young children transmit redundant information along diffusion chains? , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[16]  Frank C Keil,et al.  The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  S. Gaskins Children's Daily Activities in a Mayan Village: A Culturally Grounded Description , 2000 .

[18]  N. Akhtar,et al.  Learning words through overhearing. , 2001, Child development.

[19]  H. Rakoczy Play, games, and the development of collective intentionality. , 2007, New directions for child and adolescent development.

[20]  J. Herbert,et al.  The role of demonstrator familiarity and language cues on infant imitation from television. , 2010, Infant behavior & development.

[21]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Children’s imitation of causal action sequences is influenced by statistical and pedagogical evidence , 2011, Cognition.

[22]  M. Carpenter,et al.  Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior. , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.

[23]  Amy K. Gardiner,et al.  Guided by Intention: Preschoolers' Imitation Reflects Inferences of Causation , 2011 .

[24]  A. Whiten,et al.  Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens) , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[25]  M. Tomasello The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition , 2000 .

[26]  Mark Nielsen The social motivation for social learning , 2008 .

[27]  B. Rogoff The Cultural Nature of Human Development , 2003 .

[28]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Third-party social interaction and word learning from video. , 2011, Child development.

[29]  Mark Nielsen,et al.  Imitation in young children: when who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[30]  Barbara Rogoff,et al.  Children's attention to interactions directed to others: Guatemalan mayan and european american patterns. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[31]  N. Akhtar,et al.  Imitative learning from a third-party interaction: relations with self-recognition and perspective taking. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[32]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Emulation and "overemulation" in the social learning of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 23- and 30-month-olds. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[33]  M. Nielsen,et al.  Overimitation in Kalahari Bushman Children and the Origins of Human Cultural Cognition , 2010, Psychological science.

[34]  Nameera Akhtar,et al.  Can 18-Month-Old Infants Learn Words by Listening In on Conversations? , 2006, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[35]  Lydia M. Hopper,et al.  Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  V. Jaswal,et al.  Watch and Learn? Infants Privilege Efficiency Over Pedagogy During Imitative Learning. , 2011, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[37]  M. Carpenter,et al.  Tools, TV, and trust: introduction to the special issue on imitation in typically-developing children. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[38]  N. McGuigan The role of transmission biases in the cultural diffusion of irrelevant actions. , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.

[39]  A. Whiten,et al.  Cultures in chimpanzees , 1999, Nature.

[40]  Andrew G. Young,et al.  The hidden structure of overimitation , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[41]  B. Kenward,et al.  Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[42]  A. Whiten The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and ancestral apes , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.