Category Norms as a Function of Culture and Age: Comparisons of Item Responses to 105 Categories by American and Chinese Adults

Understanding how aging influences cognition across different cultures has been hindered by a lack of standardized, cross-referenced verbal stimuli. This study introduces a database of such item-level stimuli for both younger and older adults, in China and the United States, and makes 3 distinct contributions. First, the authors specify which item categories generalize across age and/or cultural groups, rigorously quantifying differences among them. Second, they introduce novel, powerful methods to measure between-group differences in freely generated ranked data, the rank-ordered logit model and Hellinger Affinity. Finally, a broad archive of tested, cross-linguistic stimuli is now freely available to researchers: data, similarity measures, and all stimulus materials for 105 categories and 4 culture-by-age groups, comprising over 10,000 fully translated unique item responses. The past decade has seen a marked increase in cross-cultural research in both cognitive and social psychology. Differences in cognitive and reasoning processes prevalent in Eastern and Western cultures have emerged as a topic of particular interest (e. More recently, researchers have emphasized how investigations into the interplay between aging and cultural differences in cognitive processes—particularly so between East Asian and Western cultures— can help distinguish the aspects of cognitive aging that are culturally invariant from those that depend on culture (Park, Nisbett, & Hedden, 1999). Research on cross-cultural and age differences in cognition, however, has been hampered by the lack of appropriate normed verbal materials. Given the time-consuming and costly nature of conducting norming studies, it is unsurprising that the availability of norms is limited. With respect to category norms comparing younger and older adult groups within a culture, the limited availability is further attributable to a general assumption among researchers that semantic categories remain relatively unaffected by normal aging, that is, that the structure of semantic categories does not qualitatively vary as individuals age. That semantic knowledge remains constant across the lifespan appears to be inferred primarily from two sources. First, researchers have documented a lack of age-related declines in performance on standard vocabulary tests (e.g., Salthouse, 1993); in fact, many studies have reported improvement in vocabulary scores with age (see Kausler, 1991, for a review). Second, notwithstanding the slower responses typically observed with aging (e.g., Birren & Fisher, 1995), results of semantic priming studies indicate that facilitative priming effects do not degrade across the lifespan On the one hand, it stands to reason that knowledge representations of common categories (e.g., type of fruit) remain intact if …

[1]  Sophia Rabe-Hesketh,et al.  Multilevel logistic regression for polytomous data and rankings , 2003 .

[2]  C. Stevens Memory: From Mind to Molecules , 1999, Nature Medicine.

[3]  Lillian Lee,et al.  Measures of Distributional Similarity , 1999, ACL.

[4]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Aging, culture, and cognition. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[5]  W. Parr,et al.  New Zealand norms for a subset of Battig and Montague's (1969) categories. , 1996 .

[6]  P. Schwanenflugel,et al.  Cultural Familiarity and Language Factors in the Structure of Category Knowledge , 1995 .

[7]  J. Hanley,et al.  Phonological awareness and visual skills in learning to read Chinese and English , 1995, Cognition.

[8]  Dale J. Poirier,et al.  Rank‐ordered logit models: An empirical analysis of Ontario voter preferences , 1994 .

[9]  J. J. Smith,et al.  Using ANTHOPAC 3.5 and a Spreadsheet to Compute a Free-List Salience Index , 1993 .

[10]  A. Rapoport,et al.  An Empirical Evaluation of Six Voting Procedures: Do They Really Make Any Difference? , 1993, British Journal of Political Science.

[11]  A. Agresti [A Survey of Exact Inference for Contingency Tables]: Rejoinder , 1992 .

[12]  Alan Agresti,et al.  Categorical Data Analysis , 2003 .

[13]  P. Schwanenflugel,et al.  Category typicality, cultural familiarity, and the development of category knowledge. , 1990 .

[14]  R. Heath,et al.  Category norms for australians , 1988 .

[15]  Shelby J. Haberman,et al.  A Warning on the Use of Chi-Squared Statistics with Frequency Tables with Small Expected Cell Counts , 1988 .

[16]  L L Light,et al.  Implicit and explicit memory in young and older adults. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  D. Zelterman Goodness-of-Fit Tests for Large Sparse Multinomial Distributions , 1987 .

[18]  D. Critchlow Metric Methods for Analyzing Partially Ranked Data , 1986 .

[19]  Timothy R. C. Read,et al.  Multinomial goodness-of-fit tests , 1984 .

[20]  James A. Hampton,et al.  Measures of internal category structure: A correlational analysis of normative data , 1983 .

[21]  Randall G. Chapaaan,et al.  Exploiting Rank Ordered Choice Set Data within the Stochastic Utility Model , 1982 .

[22]  L. Seiford,et al.  On the Borda-Kendall Consensus Method for Priority Ranking Problems , 1982 .

[23]  Douglas L. Nelson,et al.  Category Name and Instance Norms for 106 Categories of Various Sizes , 1982 .

[24]  K. Koehler,et al.  An Empirical Investigation of Goodness-of-Fit Statistics for Sparse Multinomials , 1980 .

[25]  D. Howard,et al.  Category norms: a comparison of the Battig and Montague (1969) norms with the responses of adults between the ages of 20 and 80. , 1980, Journal of gerontology.

[26]  C. Posnansky,et al.  Category norms for verbal items in 25 categories for children in Grades 2–6 , 1978 .

[27]  W. P. Brown,et al.  Studies in Word Listing: Testing for Group Differences in Category Norms , 1976 .

[28]  W. Montague,et al.  Category norms of verbal items in 56 categories A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms , 1969 .

[29]  W. A. Bousfield,et al.  CULTURAL NORMS FOR VERBAL ITEMS IN 43 CATEGORIES , 1957 .

[30]  H. Lin,et al.  A Cross-cultural color-naming study. Part I- Using an unconstrained method. , 2001 .

[31]  R. Nisbett,et al.  The cultural matrix of social psychology , 1998 .

[32]  J. Birren,et al.  Aging and speed of behavior: possible consequences for psychological functioning. , 1995, Annual review of psychology.

[33]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Speed and knowledge as determinants of adult age differences in verbal tasks. , 1993, Journal of gerontology.

[34]  C. Perfetti,et al.  Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a "universal" phonological principle. , 1992 .

[35]  A. Pinto Medidas de categorização : frequência de produção e de tipicidade , 1992 .

[36]  M. Bond Beyond the Chinese face : insights from psychology , 1991 .

[37]  L. Light Memory and aging: four hypotheses in search of data. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.

[38]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Category content: Findings for categories in Hebrew and a comparison to findings in the US. , 1989 .

[39]  J. Friedman,et al.  Graph-Theoretic Measures of Multivariate Association and Prediction , 1983 .

[40]  D. Kausler Experimental psychology, cognition, and human aging , 1982 .

[41]  D. McFadden Conditional logit analysis of qualitative choice behavior , 1972 .