Choking on the Money

A pernicious paradox in human motivation is the occasional reduced performance associated with tasks and situations that involve larger-than-average rewards. Three broad explanations that might account for such performance decrements are attentional competition (distraction theories), inhibition by conscious processes (explicit-monitoring theories), and excessive drive and arousal (overmotivation theories). Here, we report incentive-dependent performance decrements in humans in a reward-pursuit task; subjects were less successful in capturing a more valuable reward in a computerized maze. Concurrent functional magnetic resonance imaging revealed that increased activity in ventral midbrain, a brain area associated with incentive motivation and basic reward responding, correlated with both reduced number of captures and increased number of near-misses associated with imminent high rewards. These data cast light on the neurobiological basis of choking under pressure and are consistent with overmotivation accounts.

[1]  Nina Mazar,et al.  Large stakes and big mistakes , 2009 .

[2]  L. Pessoa On the relationship between emotion and cognition , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[3]  Marci S. DeCaro,et al.  From poor performance to success under stress: working memory, strategy selection, and mathematical problem solving under pressure. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  D. Hassabis,et al.  When Fear Is Near: Threat Imminence Elicits Prefrontal-Periaqueductal Gray Shifts in Humans , 2007, Science.

[5]  W. Schultz Behavioral dopamine signals , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.

[6]  W. Schultz,et al.  Learning-Related Human Brain Activations Reflecting Individual Finances , 2007, Neuron.

[7]  Young T. Hong,et al.  Nucleus Accumbens D2/3 Receptors Predict Trait Impulsivity and Cocaine Reinforcement , 2007, Science.

[8]  W. D. Penny,et al.  Random-Effects Analysis , 2002 .

[9]  R. Deichmann,et al.  Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex: compensation of susceptibility-induced gradients in the readout direction , 2007, Magnetic Resonance Materials in Physics, Biology and Medicine.

[10]  N. Bunzeck,et al.  Absolute Coding of Stimulus Novelty in the Human Substantia Nigra/VTA , 2006, Neuron.

[11]  H. M. Wallace,et al.  Audience support and choking under pressure: A home disadvantage? , 2005, Journal of sports sciences.

[12]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  By Carrot or by Stick: Cognitive Reinforcement Learning in Parkinsonism , 2004, Science.

[13]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[14]  R. Wise Dopamine, learning and motivation , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[15]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable Roles of Ventral and Dorsal Striatum in Instrumental Conditioning , 2004, Science.

[16]  R Turner,et al.  Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[17]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Temporal Difference Models and Reward-Related Learning in the Human Brain , 2003, Neuron.

[18]  David I. Laibson,et al.  Neuroeconomics: How Neuroscience Can Inform Economics , 2005 .

[19]  T. Robbins ADHD and Addiction , 2020, Nature Medicine.

[20]  Sham M. Kakade,et al.  Opponent interactions between serotonin and dopamine , 2002, Neural Networks.

[21]  Geoffrey B. Sprinkle,et al.  The effects of monetary incentives on effort and task performance: theories, evidence, and a framework for research , 2002 .

[22]  W. Penny,et al.  Random-Effects Analysis , 2002 .

[23]  M. Ashcraft,et al.  The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Attention and performance. , 2001, Annual review of psychology.

[25]  Charles R. Yang,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortical output neurons to the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and their responses to burst‐patterned stimulation of the VTA: Neuroanatomical and in vivo electrophysiological analyses , 1999, Synapse.

[26]  P S Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Increased dopamine turnover in the prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance in rats and monkeys. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  Peter Norvig,et al.  Artificial Intelligence: A Modern Approach , 1995 .

[28]  John Kelly,et al.  Artificial intelligence - a modern myth , 1993, Ellis Horwood series in artificial intelligence.

[29]  R. M. Nideffer,et al.  Psyched To Win , 1992 .

[30]  H. Sigall,et al.  Self-Consciousness, Self-presentation, and Performance Under Pressure: Who Chokes, and When? , 1991 .

[31]  R. Sorrentino,et al.  Achievement, affiliation, and group incentives: A test of the overmotivation hypothesis , 1986 .

[32]  R. Baumeister Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. , 1984, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  D. Landers,et al.  The arousal-performance relationship revisited. , 1980, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[34]  C. Carver,et al.  Self-focusing effects of dispositional self-consciousness, mirror presence, and audience presence. , 1978 .

[35]  J. Easterbrook The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. , 1959, Psychological review.