The influence of sensory information on patch residence time in wolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae)
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] G. Uetz,et al. SURVIVORSHIP OF WOLF SPIDERS (LYCOSIDAE ) REARED ON DIFFERENT DIET S , 1992 .
[2] E. T. Segura,et al. The tale of the screaming hairy armadillo, the guinea pig and the marginal value theorem , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[3] David W. Stephens,et al. On the economics of sit-and-wait foraging: site selection and assessment , 1995 .
[4] G. Uetz,et al. Foraging strategies of spiders. , 1992, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[5] D. H. Morse,et al. Experimental and Observational Studies of Patch Choice at Different Scales by the Crab Spider Misumena Vatia , 1982 .
[6] G. Sonerud,et al. Auditory prey location in a pause—travel predator: search height, search time, and attack range of Tengmalm's owls (Aegolius funereus) , 1992 .
[7] T. Valone,et al. Patch estimation by group foragers: what information is used? , 1993, Animal Behaviour.
[8] G. Uetz,et al. Communication via substratum-coupled stridulation and reproductive isolation in wolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae) , 1983, Animal Behaviour.
[9] S. Healy,et al. Spatial memory of paridae: comparison of a storing and a non-storing species, the coal tit, Parus ater, and the great tit, P. major , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[10] A I Houston,et al. Memory and the efficient use of information. , 1987, Journal of theoretical biology.
[11] F. Barth,et al. Neurobiology of Arachnids , 1985, Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
[12] J. Krebs,et al. Starlings exploiting patches: the effect of recent experience on foraging decisions , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[13] S. Riechert. Thoughts on the Ecological Significance of Spiders , 1974 .
[14] H. Shaffer,et al. Aquatic prey capture in ambystomatid salamanders: Patterns of variation in muscle activity , 1985, Journal of morphology.
[15] T. Caraco,et al. Risk‐Sensitivity: Foraging Mode in an Ambush Predator , 1986 .
[16] D. Wise. Spiders in Ecological Webs: References , 1993 .
[17] G. Uetz,et al. Acoustic Communication and Reproductive Isolation in Two Species of Wolf Spiders , 1981, Science.
[18] E. Charnov. Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. , 1976, Theoretical population biology.
[19] S. Riechert. Decisions in Multiple Goal Contexts: Habitat Selection of the Spider, Agelenopsis aperta (Gertsch) , 1985 .
[20] Kinya Nishimura,et al. Decision Making of a Sit-and-Wait Forager in an Uncertain Environment: Learning and Memory Load , 1994, The American Naturalist.
[21] J. Rovner. EVIDENCE FOR IDIOTHETICALLY CONTROLLED TURNS AN D EXTRAOCULAR PHOTORECEPTION IN LYCOSID SPIDER S , 1991 .
[22] Michael F. Land,et al. The Morphology and Optics of Spider Eyes , 1985 .
[23] J. McNamara. Optimal patch use in a stochastic environment , 1982 .
[24] B. J. Cockrell,et al. Predator ingestion rate and its bearing on feeding time and the theory of oprimal diets , 1978 .
[25] J. Krebs,et al. Hunting by expectation or optimal foraging: A study of patch use by chickadees , 1974 .
[26] A. F. Bennett,et al. Foraging Strategy and Metabolic Rate in Spiders , 1980 .
[27] K. Nishimura. Optimal patch residence time of a sit-and-wait forager , 1991 .
[28] W. Tietjen. TESTS FOR OLFACTORY COMMUNICATION IN FOU R SPECIES OF WOLF SPIDERS (ARANEAE, LYCOSIDAE) 1 , 1979 .
[29] A. F. Bennett,et al. Muscle recruitment during terrestrial locomotion : how speed and temperature affect fibre type use in a lizard , 1990 .
[30] S. Young,et al. Visually guided feeding behaviour in the filter feeding cladoceran, Daphaia magna , 1987, Animal Behaviour.
[31] J. Endler. Signals, Signal Conditions, and the Direction of Evolution , 1992, The American Naturalist.
[32] G. Uetz. Habitat structure and spider foraging , 1991 .
[33] Michael J. Ford,et al. Locomotory activity and the predation strategy of the of the wolf-spider Pardosa amentata (Clerck) (Lycosidae) , 1978, Animal Behaviour.
[34] P. Wainwright. Prey Processing in Haemulid Fishes: Patterns of Variation in Pharyngeal Jaw Muscle Activity , 1989 .
[35] A. Sih. Optimal Foraging: Partial Consumption of Prey , 1980, The American Naturalist.
[36] G. Uetz,et al. THE INHERITANCE OF COURTSHIP BEHAVIOR AND ITS ROLE AS A REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATING MECHANISM IN TWO SPECIES OF SCHIZOCOSA WOLF SPIDERS (ARANEAE; LYCOSIDAE) , 1986, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.
[37] Enigmatic cohabitation in bowl and doily spiders, Frontinella pyramitela (Araneae, Linyphiidae) , 1989, Animal Behaviour.
[38] A. Cady. MICROHABITAT SELECTION AND LOCOMOTOR ACTIVIT Y OF SCHIZOCOSA OCREATA (WALCKENAER) (ARANEAE : LYCOSIDAE) , 1984 .
[39] K. Miyashita. Effects of Locomotory Activity, Temperature and Hunger on the Respiratory Rate of Lycosa T-insignita BOES. et STR. (Araneae : Lycosidae) , 1969 .
[40] W. Aspey. Wolf spider sociobiology: I. Agonistic display and dominance-subordinance relations in adult male Schizocosa crassipes. , 1977, Behaviour.
[41] J. H. Zar,et al. Biostatistical Analysis (5th Edition) , 1984 .
[42] Robert A. Johnson. Learning, Memory, and Foraging Efficiency in Two Species of Desert Seed‐Harvester Ants , 1991 .
[43] Richard F. Green,et al. Bayesian birds: A simple example of Oaten's stochastic model of optimal foraging , 1980 .
[44] Wolf spider sociobiology: II. Density parameters influencing agonistic behavior in Schizocosa crassipes. , 1977, Behaviour.
[45] G. Uetz,et al. Sexual selection, male morphology, and the efficacy of courtship signalling in two wolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae) , 1996, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[46] A. Houston,et al. Optimal foraging and learning , 1985 .
[47] Thomas J. Valone,et al. Measuring Patch Assessment Abilities of Desert Granivores , 1989 .
[48] F. Barth. Chapter 3 Spiders and Vibratory Signals: Sensory Reception and Behavioral Significance , 1982 .
[49] Thomas J. Valone,et al. Bayesian and prescient assessment: foraging with pre-harvest information , 1991, Animal Behaviour.
[50] S. Riechert,et al. Spiders as Biological Control Agents , 1984 .
[51] J. Rovner,et al. Nocturnal capture of fireflies by lycosid spiders: visual versus vibratory stimuli , 1988, Animal Behaviour.