Measuring the fading consciousness in the human brain.

PURPOSE OF REVIEW Recent neuroimaging studies have provided novel insights on residual brain function in patients with disorders of consciousness, but also raised a number of ethical issues concerning the clinical management of these patients. RECENT FINDINGS Clinical studies have rated the Coma Recovery Scale as the most appropriate scale to accurately differentiate patients in a vegetative state from patients in a minimally conscious state. At the population level, a number of neuroimaging studies have provided evidence for more preserved brain activity patterns and cerebral tissue integrity in minimally conscious as compared to vegetative-state patients. However, the use of neuroimaging techniques to diagnose consciousness at the single-patient level remains challenging. In particular, it has been shown that whereas command-following functional MRI paradigms may sometimes detect residual awareness in patients that are behaviorally unresponsive, they can also produce negative results in patients that are communicative at the bedside. SUMMARY There is an urgent need of validation of functional MRI active paradigms on larger patient populations before they can be used in clinical routine. Further research on neural correlates of consciousness should hopefully allow using passive paradigms to assess the patients' conscious state without requiring their active collaboration.

[1]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Perception of pain in the minimally conscious state with PET activation: an observational study , 2008, The Lancet Neurology.

[2]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. , 2006, Science.

[3]  C. Fischer,et al.  Event-related potentials (MMN and novelty P3) in permanent vegetative or minimally conscious states , 2010, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[4]  M. Boly,et al.  Consciousness and cerebral baseline activity fluctuations , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[5]  Steve Majerus,et al.  Detecting Consciousness in a Total Locked-in Syndrome: an Active Event-related Paradigm Detecting Consciousness in a Total Lis , 2022 .

[6]  N. Schiff,et al.  Dissociations between behavioural and functional magnetic resonance imaging-based evaluations of cognitive function after brain injury. , 2011, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  Guy B. Williams,et al.  Aetiological differences in neuroanatomy of the vegetative state: insights from diffusion tensor imaging and functional implications , 2010, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[8]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Cerebral response to patient's own name in the vegetative and minimally conscious states , 2007, Neurology.

[9]  Qiyong Gong,et al.  Anterior cingulate activity and the self in disorders of consciousness , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[10]  Steve Majerus,et al.  Assessment and detection of pain in noncommunicative severely brain-injured patients , 2010, Expert review of neurotherapeutics.

[11]  L. Santoro,et al.  Late recovery after traumatic, anoxic, or hemorrhagic long-lasting vegetative state , 2010, Neurology.

[12]  J. Giacino,et al.  The JFK Coma Recovery Scale-Revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. , 2004, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[13]  T Sarraf,et al.  Long-term outcomes of chronic minimally conscious and vegetative states , 2010, Neurology.

[14]  Keith A. Johnson,et al.  Cortical Hubs Revealed by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity: Mapping, Assessment of Stability, and Relation to Alzheimer's Disease , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  P. Nachev,et al.  Covert cognition in the persistent vegetative state , 2010, Progress in Neurobiology.

[16]  Marcello Massimini,et al.  Theoretical approaches to the diagnosis of altered states of consciousness. , 2009, Progress in brain research.

[17]  Steven Laureys,et al.  The Nociception Coma Scale: A new tool to assess nociception in disorders of consciousness , 2010, PAIN.

[18]  Olivia Gosseries,et al.  Assessment of consciousness with electrophysiological and neurological imaging techniques , 2011, Current opinion in critical care.

[19]  M. Boly,et al.  Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-communicative brain-damaged patients. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  Habib Benali,et al.  Relation between brain lesion location and clinical outcome in patients with severe traumatic brain injury: A diffusion tensor imaging study using voxel‐based approaches , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[21]  Walter G Sannita,et al.  Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome , 2010, BMC medicine.

[22]  G. Tononi Consciousness as Integrated Information: a Provisional Manifesto , 2008, The Biological Bulletin.

[23]  M. Boly,et al.  Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. , 2010, The New England journal of medicine.

[24]  Anthony G. Hudetz,et al.  Specific and nonspecific thalamocortical functional connectivity in normal and vegetative states , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[25]  M. Boly,et al.  Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: Clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment , 2009, BMC neurology.

[26]  Ross Zafonte,et al.  Assessment scales for disorders of consciousness: evidence-based recommendations for clinical practice and research. , 2010, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[27]  Marcello Massimini,et al.  A perturbational approach for evaluating the brain's capacity for consciousness. , 2009, Progress in brain research.

[28]  Maxime Bonjean,et al.  “Relevance vector machine” consciousness classifier applied to cerebral metabolism of vegetative and locked-in patients , 2011, NeuroImage.

[29]  M. Boly,et al.  Functional connectivity in the default network during resting state is preserved in a vegetative but not in a brain dead patient , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[30]  Fabrice Wendling,et al.  Impaired consciousness during temporal lobe seizures is related to increased long-distance cortical-subcortical synchronization. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  Montserrat Bernabeu,et al.  Reductions of thalamic volume and regional shape changes in the vegetative and the minimally conscious states. , 2010, Journal of neurotrauma.

[32]  J. Régis,et al.  The role of corticothalamic coupling in human temporal lobe epilepsy. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  M. Boly,et al.  Breakdown of within- and between-network Resting State Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Connectivity during Propofol-induced Loss of Consciousness , 2010, Anesthesiology.

[34]  J. Changeux,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.5 May 2006 Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy , 2022 .

[35]  J. Hirsch,et al.  A network approach to assessing cognition in disorders of consciousness , 2010, Neurology.

[36]  G. Tononi,et al.  Breakdown in cortical effective connectivity during midazolam-induced loss of consciousness , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[37]  F. Chollet,et al.  Wakefulness and loss of awareness , 2010, Neurology.

[38]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Diffusion weighted imaging distinguishes the vegetative state from the minimally conscious state , 2011, NeuroImage.

[39]  O. Sporns,et al.  Mapping the Structural Core of Human Cerebral Cortex , 2008, PLoS biology.

[40]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Towards the routine use of brain imaging to aid the clinical diagnosis of disorders of consciousness. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  J. Brewer,et al.  Functional Brain Imaging in the Clinical Assessment of Consciousness , 2010, PLoS Biology.

[42]  Steven Laureys,et al.  The changing spectrum of coma , 2008, Nature Clinical Practice Neurology.