Embodiment and Presence in Virtual Reality After Stroke. A Comparative Study With Healthy Subjects

The ability of virtual reality (VR) to recreate controlled, immersive, and interactive environments that provide intensive and customized exercises has motivated its therapeutic use after stroke. Interaction and bodily presence in VR-based interventions is usually mediated through virtual selves, which synchronously represent body movements or responses to events on external input devices. Embodied self-representations in the virtual world not only provide an anchor for visuomotor tasks, but their morphologies can have behavioral implications. While research has focused on the underlying subjective mechanisms of exposure to VR on healthy individuals, the transference of these findings to individuals with stroke is not evident and remains unexplored, which could affect the experience and, ultimately, the clinical effectiveness of neurorehabilitation interventions. This study determined and compared the sense of embodiment and presence elicited by a virtual environment under different perspectives and levels of immersion in healthy subjects and individuals with stroke. Forty-six healthy subjects and 32 individuals with stroke embodied a gender-matched neutral avatar in a virtual environment that was displayed in a first-person perspective with a head-mounted display and in a third-person perspective with a screen, and the participants were asked to interact in a virtual task for 10 min under each condition in counterbalanced order, and to complete two questionnaires about the sense of embodiment and presence experienced during the interaction. The sense of body-ownership, self-location, and presence were more vividly experienced in a first-person than in a third-person perspective by both healthy subjects (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.212; p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.101; p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.401, respectively) and individuals with stroke (p = 0.019, ηp2 = 0.070; p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.135; p = 0.014, ηp2 = 0.077, respectively). In contrast, no agency perspective-related differences were found in any group. All measures were consistently higher for healthy controls than for individuals with stroke, but differences between groups only reached statistical significance in presence under the first-person condition (p < 0.010, ηp2 = 0.084). In spite of these differences, the participants experienced a vivid sense of embodiment and presence in almost all conditions. These results provide first evidence that, although less intensively, embodiment and presence are similarly experienced by individuals who have suffered a stroke and by healthy individuals, which could support the vividness of their experience and, consequently, the effectiveness of VR-based interventions.

[1]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[2]  Carrie Heeter,et al.  Being There: The Subjective Experience of Presence , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[3]  B. E. Maki,et al.  Measuring balance in the elderly: validation of an instrument. , 1992, Canadian journal of public health = Revue canadienne de sante publique.

[4]  Carl Machover,et al.  Virtual reality , 1994, IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications.

[5]  S. Aglioti,et al.  The body in the brain: neural bases of corporeal awareness , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[6]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.

[7]  Mel Slater,et al.  A Virtual Presence Counter , 2000, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[8]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. , 2000, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[9]  Rosa María Baños,et al.  Immersion and Emotion: Their Impact on the Sense of Presence , 2004, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[10]  O. Blanke,et al.  Neural mechanisms of embodiment: asomatognosia due to premotor cortex damage. , 2006, Archives of neurology.

[11]  Daniel Thalmann,et al.  The benefits of third-person perspective in virtual and augmented reality? , 2006, VRST '06.

[12]  Dorothée Legrand The Bodily Self: The Sensori-Motor Roots of Pre-Reflective Self-Consciousness , 2006 .

[13]  Yvonne de Kort,et al.  Is This My Hand I See Before Me? The Rubber Hand Illusion in Reality, Virtual Reality, and Mixed Reality , 2006, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[14]  J. Bailenson,et al.  The Proteus Effect: The Effect of Transformed Self-Representation on Behavior , 2007 .

[15]  Mariano Alcañiz Raya,et al.  Affective Interactions Using Virtual Reality: The Link between Presence and Emotions , 2007, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[16]  Patrice L. (Tamar) Weiss,et al.  Evaluation of virtual shopping in the VMall: Comparison of post-stroke participants to healthy control groups , 2007, Disability and rehabilitation.

[17]  J. Bradshaw,et al.  Mechanisms underlying embodiment, disembodiment and loss of embodiment , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[18]  Marjolein P. M. Kammers,et al.  What is embodiment? A psychometric approach , 2008, Cognition.

[19]  Justin M. Harris,et al.  If I Were You: Perceptual Illusion of Body Swapping , 2008, PloS one.

[20]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Inducing Illusory Ownership of a Virtual Body , 2009, Front. Neurosci..

[21]  C. Moore,et al.  Movement and the Rubber Hand Illusion , 2009, Perception.

[22]  Mel Slater,et al.  Place illusion and plausibility can lead to realistic behaviour in immersive virtual environments , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  First Person Experience of Body Transfer in Virtual Reality , 2010, PloS one.

[24]  C. Colomer,et al.  Effectiveness of a Wii balance board-based system (eBaViR) for balance rehabilitation: a pilot randomized clinical trial in patients with acquired brain injury , 2011, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[25]  Daniel Thalmann,et al.  Quantifying Effects of Exposure to the Third and First-Person Perspectives in Virtual-Reality-Based Training , 2010, IEEE Transactions on Learning Technologies.

[26]  F. Vignemont Embodiment, ownership and disownership , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[27]  H. Johansen-Berg,et al.  Ventral Premotor Cortex May Be Required for Dynamic Changes in the Feeling of Limb Ownership: A Lesion Study , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  M. Levin,et al.  Virtual Reality in Stroke Rehabilitation: A Meta-Analysis and Implications for Clinicians , 2011, Stroke.

[29]  Valeria I. Petkova,et al.  The Perspective Matters! Multisensory Integration in Ego-Centric Reference Frames Determines Full-Body Ownership , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[30]  F. de Vignemont Embodiment, ownership and disownership. , 2011, Consciousness and cognition.

[31]  Mel Slater,et al.  Is my hand connected to my body? The impact of body continuity and arm alignment on the virtual hand illusion , 2011, Cognitive Neurodynamics.

[32]  Saniye Tugba Bulu,et al.  Place presence, social presence, co-presence, and satisfaction in virtual worlds , 2012, Comput. Educ..

[33]  J. Ferri,et al.  Clinical usefulness of the Spanish version of the Mississippi Aphasia Screening Test (MASTsp): validation in stroke patients , 2012 .

[34]  M. Slater,et al.  The building blocks of the full body ownership illusion , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[35]  M. Slater,et al.  Illusory ownership of a virtual child body causes overestimation of object sizes and implicit attitude changes , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  A. Clark,et al.  Decomposing the Will , 2013 .

[37]  Courtney G. E. Hilderman,et al.  Virtual Reality Therapy for Adults Post-Stroke: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis Exploring Virtual Environments and Commercial Games in Therapy , 2014, PloS one.

[38]  E. Noé,et al.  Improvement in balance using a virtual reality-based stepping exercise: a randomized controlled trial involving individuals with chronic stroke , 2015, Clinical rehabilitation.

[39]  Mariano Alcañiz Raya,et al.  Tracking Systems for Virtual Rehabilitation: Objective Performance vs. Subjective Experience. A Practical Scenario , 2015, Sensors.

[40]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Over my fake body: body ownership illusions for studying the multisensory basis of own-body perception , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[41]  Ronan Boulic,et al.  Characterizing embodied interaction in First and Third Person Perspective viewpoints , 2015, 2015 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (3DUI).

[42]  Lise Worthen-Chaudhari,et al.  Effectiveness, usability, and cost-benefit of a virtual reality-based telerehabilitation program for balance recovery after stroke: a randomized controlled trial. , 2015, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[43]  Snezana Stupar-Rutenfrans,et al.  Observe or Participate: The Effect of Point-Of-View on Presence and Enjoyment in 360 Degree Movies for Head Mounted Displays , 2015, AmI.

[44]  F. Garbarini,et al.  Are Movements Necessary for the Sense of Body Ownership? Evidence from the Rubber Hand Illusion in Pure Hemiplegic Patients , 2015, PLoS ONE.

[45]  C. Cipriani,et al.  The rubber foot illusion , 2015, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[46]  B. Hommel,et al.  The role of agency for perceived ownership in the virtual hand illusion , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[47]  J. Deutsch,et al.  Virtual Reality for Sensorimotor Rehabilitation Post Stroke: Design Principles and Evidence , 2016 .

[48]  E. Noé,et al.  Effect of a mixed reality-based intervention on arm, hand, and finger function on chronic stroke , 2016, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[49]  David Swapp,et al.  An ‘In the Wild’ Experiment on Presence and Embodiment using Consumer Virtual Reality Equipment , 2016, IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics.

[50]  Xiuguo Han,et al.  Virtual reality for improving balance in patients after stroke: A systematic review and meta-analysis , 2016, Clinical rehabilitation.

[51]  Simon Richir,et al.  First- and Third-Person Perspectives in Immersive Virtual Environments: Presence and Performance Analysis of Embodied Users , 2017, Front. Robot. AI.

[52]  C. Myers,et al.  Beyond Behavioral Inhibition: A Computer Avatar Task Designed to Assess Behavioral Inhibition Extends to Harm Avoidance , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[53]  B. Lange,et al.  Virtual reality for stroke rehabilitation. , 2015, The Cochrane database of systematic reviews.

[54]  E. Noé,et al.  Body schema plasticity after stroke: Subjective and neurophysiological correlates of the rubber hand illusion , 2017, Neuropsychologia.

[55]  Holger Regenbrecht,et al.  Mixed Voxel Reality: Presence and Embodiment in Low Fidelity, Visually Coherent, Mixed Reality Environments , 2017, 2017 IEEE International Symposium on Mixed and Augmented Reality (ISMAR).

[56]  K. Caeyenberghs,et al.  What do randomized controlled trials say about virtual rehabilitation in stroke? A systematic literature review and meta-analysis of upper-limb and cognitive outcomes , 2018, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[57]  Mariano Alcañiz,et al.  Reliability and comparison of Kinect-based methods for estimating spatiotemporal gait parameters of healthy and post-stroke individuals. , 2018, Journal of biomechanics.

[58]  Holger Regenbrecht,et al.  Is That Me?—Embodiment and Body Perception with an Augmented Reality Mirror , 2018, 2018 IEEE International Symposium on Mixed and Augmented Reality Adjunct (ISMAR-Adjunct).

[59]  R. Lloréns,et al.  Comparison of Oculus Rift and HTC Vive: Feasibility for Virtual Reality-Based Exploration, Navigation, Exergaming, and Rehabilitation. , 2018, Games for health journal.

[60]  R. Teasell,et al.  Post-Stroke Cognitive Disorders , 2022 .