Vocal Learning in the Functionally Referential Food Grunts of Chimpanzees

One standout feature of human language is our ability to reference external objects and events with socially learned symbols, or words. Exploring the phylogenetic origins of this capacity is therefore key to a comprehensive understanding of the evolution of language. While non-human primates can produce vocalizations that refer to external objects in the environment, it is generally accepted that their acoustic structure is fixed and a product of arousal states. Indeed, it has been argued that the apparent lack of flexible control over the structure of referential vocalizations represents a key discontinuity with language. Here, we demonstrate vocal learning in the acoustic structure of referential food grunts in captive chimpanzees. We found that, following the integration of two groups of adult chimpanzees, the acoustic structure of referential food grunts produced for a specific food converged over 3 years. Acoustic convergence arose independently of preference for the food, and social network analyses indicated this only occurred after strong affiliative relationships were established between the original subgroups. We argue that these data represent the first evidence of non-human animals actively modifying and socially learning the structure of a meaningful referential vocalization from conspecifics. Our findings indicate that primate referential call structure is not simply determined by arousal and that the socially learned nature of referential words in humans likely has ancient evolutionary origins.

[1]  T. Geissmann Duet‐splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs , 2002, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[2]  J. Mitani,et al.  Social Factors Influence the Acoustic Variability in the Long‐distance Calls of Male Chimpanzees , 2010 .

[3]  Charles T. Snowdon,et al.  Pygmy Marmosets modify call structure when paired , 1999 .

[4]  Mariano Ruiz Espejo,et al.  Review of Statistical computing: An introduction to data analysis using S-PLUS by M. J. Crawley, John Wiley & Sons, Chichester 2002 , 2003 .

[5]  Felipe De Brigard,et al.  The Origins of Meaning: Language in the Light of Evolution , 2009 .

[6]  S. Kirby,et al.  The emergence of linguistic structure: an overview of the iterated learning model , 2002 .

[7]  T. Geissmann Mate Change Enhances Duetting Activity in the Siamang Gibbon (Hylobates Syndactulus) , 1986 .

[8]  Klaus Zuberbühler,et al.  Chimpanzee food calls are directed at specific individuals , 2013, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  D. Lusseau,et al.  Quantifying the influence of sociality on population structure in bottlenose dolphins. , 2006, The Journal of animal ecology.

[10]  E. D. Brown,et al.  Social influences on vocal development: What birds with complex social relationships can tell us about vocal learning: Vocal sharing in avian groups , 1997 .

[11]  P. Tyack Convergence of calls as animals form social bonds, active compensation for noisy communication channels, and the evolution of vocal learning in mammals. , 2008, Journal of comparative psychology.

[12]  Hal Whitehead,et al.  Analyzing Animal Societies: Quantitative Methods for Vertebrate Social Analysis , 2008 .

[13]  J. Mitani,et al.  Chorusing and Call Convergence in Chimpanzees: Tests of Three Hypotheses , 1998 .

[14]  Simon Kirby,et al.  Iterated Learning: A Framework for the Emergence of Language , 2003, Artificial Life.

[15]  Denise Brandão de Oliveira e Britto,et al.  The faculty of language , 2007 .

[16]  Klaus Zuberbühler,et al.  Food-associated calls in chimpanzees: responses to food types or food preferences? , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  M. Ryan,et al.  What do animal signals mean? , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? , 2002, Science.

[19]  Martine Hausberger,et al.  Song sharing reflects the social organization in a captive group of European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). , 1995 .

[20]  Michael J. Crawley,et al.  Statistical Computing: An Introduction to Data Analysis using S-Plus , 2002 .

[21]  Charles T. Snowdon,et al.  Pygmy marmosets, Cebuella pygmaea, modify vocal structure in response to changed social environment , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  G. Striedter,et al.  Male vocal imitation produces call convergence during pair bonding in budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulatus , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[23]  M. Newman Analysis of weighted networks. , 2004, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.

[24]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Infants' reliance on a social criterion for establishing word-object relations. , 1996, Child development.

[25]  J. Faraway Linear Models with R , 2014 .

[26]  F. Clark Space to choose: network analysis of social preferences in a captive chimpanzee community, and implications for management , 2011, American journal of primatology.

[27]  E. van de Waal,et al.  Potent Social Learning and Conformity Shape a Wild Primate’s Foraging Decisions , 2013, Science.

[28]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer , 2002 .

[29]  Klaus Zuberbühler,et al.  Functionally Referential Communication in a Chimpanzee , 2005, Current Biology.

[30]  E. Dietz Permutation Tests for Association Between Two Distance Matrices , 1983 .

[31]  Hal Whitehead,et al.  SOCPROG programs: analysing animal social structures , 2009, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[32]  M. Tomasello Origins of human communication , 2008 .

[33]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Diana monkey long-distance calls: messages for conspecifics and predators , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[34]  I. Cuthill,et al.  Effect size, confidence interval and statistical significance: a practical guide for biologists , 2007, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[35]  Brandon C. Wheeler,et al.  Functionally referential signals: A promising paradigm whose time has passed , 2012, Evolutionary anthropology.

[36]  W. Fitch,et al.  Social Cognition and the Evolution of Language: Constructing Cognitive Phylogenies , 2010, Neuron.

[37]  E. Morton On the Occurrence and Significance of Motivation-Structural Rules in Some Bird and Mammal Sounds , 1977, The American Naturalist.

[38]  S. Nowicki,et al.  Vocal plasticity in captive black-capped chickadees: the acoustic basis and rate of call convergence , 1989, Animal Behaviour.

[39]  R. Wrangham,et al.  Does learning affect the structure of vocalizations in chimpanzees? , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[40]  P. Slater,et al.  Vocal Learning in Mammals , 1997 .

[41]  A. Whiten,et al.  Network Analysis of Social Changes in a Captive Chimpanzee Community Following the Successful Integration of Two Adult Groups , 2013, American journal of primatology.

[42]  Andrew N. Radford,et al.  The development of alarm call behaviour in mammals and birds , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[43]  K. Slocombe,et al.  Primate Vocal Communication: A Useful Tool for Understanding Human Speech and Language Evolution? , 2011, Human biology.

[44]  P. Marler,et al.  Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate , 1980, Animal Behaviour.