Disruption of delayed memory for a sequence of spatial locations following CA1- or CA3-lesions of the dorsal hippocampus

Axon-sparing neurotoxic lesions of CA1 or CA3 were produced in the dorsal hippocampus to test dissociative lesion effects on spatial working memory for sequential items. Rats were required to remember four different sections sequentially presented on a newly devised maze (i.e., Tulum maze) during a study phase. Each section was cued by a unique object that was specifically associated with each location within the section during the study phase. Following a 15-s delay and during the test phase, rats were required to revisit the location within a section randomly chosen among the previously visited sections in the absence of the cued object. Both CA1 and CA3 lesions similarly disrupted accurate relocation of a previously visited place. However, differential effects of the CA1 and CA3 lesions were observed in serial position curves. CA3-lesions disrupted performance for the first three serial positions, but did not disrupt performance for the last serial position (recency). In contrast, CA1-lesions disrupted performance for all serial positions. The results suggest that temporal separation of spatial memory may depend on the conjoint function of CA1 and CA3 of the hippocampus with a disruption of a spatial pattern completion process following CA3 lesions and a disruption of a temporal pattern separation process following a CA1 lesion.

[1]  R. Kesner,et al.  Serial position curves in rats: automatic versus effortful information processing. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[2]  R P Kesner,et al.  Memory for spatial location as a function of temporal lag in rats: role of hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. , 1994, Behavioral and neural biology.

[3]  R. Kesner,et al.  Differential contributions of dorsal hippocampal subregions to memory acquisition and retrieval in contextual fear‐conditioning , 2004, Hippocampus.

[4]  Paul E. Gilbert,et al.  Testing neural network models of memory with behavioral experiments , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[5]  M. Moser,et al.  Distributed Encoding and Retrieval of Spatial Memory in the Hippocampus , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Inah Lee,et al.  Differential roles of dorsal hippocampal subregions in spatial working memory with short versus intermediate delay. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  R. Kesner,et al.  The role of the CA3 subregion of the dorsal hippocampus in spatial pattern completion in the rat , 2005, Hippocampus.

[8]  Paul E. Gilbert,et al.  A Behavioral Assessment of Hippocampal Function Based on a Subregional Analysis , 2004, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[9]  G. Paxinos The Rat nervous system , 1985 .

[10]  J. Lisman,et al.  Storage, recall, and novelty detection of sequences by the hippocampus: Elaborating on the SOCRATIC model to account for normal and aberrant effects of dopamine , 2001, Hippocampus.

[11]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Theta phase precession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences , 1996, Hippocampus.

[12]  Raymond P. Kesner,et al.  Serial position curves for item (spatial location) information: role of the dorsal hippocampal formation and medial septum , 1988, Brain Research.

[13]  L. Jarrard On the use of ibotenic acid to lesion selectively different components of the hippocampal formation , 1989, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[14]  Inah Lee,et al.  Time-Dependent Relationship between the Dorsal Hippocampus and the Prefrontal Cortex in Spatial Memory , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  The hippocampus as an associator of discontiguous events , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[16]  W B Levy,et al.  A sequence predicting CA3 is a flexible associator that learns and uses context to solve hippocampal‐like tasks , 1996, Hippocampus.

[17]  J. Seamans,et al.  Selective Roles for Hippocampal, Prefrontal Cortical, and Ventral Striatal Circuits in Radial-Arm Maze Tasks With or Without a Delay , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  R. Morris,et al.  Spatial learning with a minislab in the dorsal hippocampus. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  H. Eichenbaum The hippocampus and declarative memory: cognitive mechanisms and neural codes , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  P. Somogyi,et al.  The hippocampal CA3 network: An in vivo intracellular labeling study , 1994, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[21]  R. Kesner,et al.  Encoding versus retrieval of spatial memory: Double dissociation between the dentate gyrus and the perforant path inputs into CA3 in the dorsal hippocampus , 2004, Hippocampus.

[22]  L. Squire,et al.  Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  W. Cowan,et al.  An autoradiographic study of the organization of the efferet connections of the hippocampal formation in the rat , 1977, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[24]  Inah Lee,et al.  Anatomical characteristics and three‐dimensional model of the dog dorsal lateral geniculate body , 1999, The Anatomical record.

[25]  R. O. Hopkins,et al.  Item and Order Recognition Memory in Subjects with Hypoxic Brain Injury , 1995, Brain and Cognition.

[26]  S J Mizumori,et al.  Hippocampal Representational Organization and Spatial Context , 1999, Hippocampus.

[27]  Inah Lee,et al.  Differential contribution of NMDA receptors in hippocampal subregions to spatial working memory , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  Albert K. Lee,et al.  Memory of Sequential Experience in the Hippocampus during Slow Wave Sleep , 2002, Neuron.

[29]  Paul E. Gilbert,et al.  Dissociating hippocampal subregions: A double dissociation between dentate gyrus and CA1 , 2001, Hippocampus.