"Compensatory Automaticity: Unconscious volition is not an oxymoron"

Nach fast drei Jahrzehnten Forschung über Automatismus und Konstruktaktivierung ist eindeutig gezeigt worden, dass vieles im menschlichen Leben ohne Bewusstsein und ohne Beabsichtigung geschieht. Laut kognitiver Psychologie werden automatische Prozesse unvermeidlich durch die Gegenwart eines bestimmten Umweltreizes ausgelöst. Sie verlaufen schnell und mühelos und sind nicht korrigierbar. Auch in der Sozialpsychologie hat das Konzept des Automatismus einen wichtigen Platz eingenommen, jedoch wurden einige der getroffenen Annahmen in Frage gestellt. Neuere Untersuchungen ergaben, dass automatische Antworten zwar nicht beabsichtigt, aber doch vermeidbar wären. Es scheint, dass unbewusste Vigilanz (Wachsamkeit) für Fehler zu korrigierenden Prozessen führen kann, welche auch unbewusst und ohne Absicht funktionieren. Wir vermuten also, dass das Unbewusste auch eigene Verarbeitungsziele hat (z.B. Genauigkeit), auf Gefahren für die Erreichung dieser Ziele aufmerksam machen kann und diese Gefahren zu kompensieren versucht. Man könnte dies als „kompensatorischen Automatismus“ bezeichnen, sprich: strategisches, unbewusstes Kompensieren für nicht beabsichtigte Gedanken, Gefühle und Verhaltensweisen. Manchen mag dies paradox erscheinen, weil Automatismus oft mit Kontrolloder Intentionsverlust gleichgesetzt wird. Nichtsdestotrotz muss man die Möglichkeit in Betracht ziehen, dass Absicht auf verschiedenen Bewusstseinsebenen operiert. Zum Beispiel können unbewusste Absichten (Ziele), wenn sie offensichtlich an ihrer Ausführung behindert werden, von automatischen Prozessen unterstützt und beschützt werden.

[1]  D. Dutton,et al.  Threat of own prejudice and reverse discrimination in interracial situations. , 1973 .

[2]  P. Katz 4 – The Acquisition of Racial Attitudes in Children , 1976 .

[3]  Paula M. Niedenthal,et al.  Implicit perception of affective information , 1990 .

[4]  M. Banaji,et al.  When fair is foul and foul is fair: reverse priming in automatic evaluation. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[5]  Norbert Schwarz,et al.  The Smell of Bias: What Instigates Correction Processes in Social Judgments? , 1998 .

[6]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. , 1977 .

[7]  Duane T. Wegener,et al.  Flexible correction processes in social judgment: the role of naive theories in corrections for perceived bias. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[8]  Duane T. Wegener,et al.  Flexible Correction Processes in Social Judgment: Correcting for Context-Induced Contrast , 1993 .

[9]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The Automatic Evaluation Effect: Unconditional Automatic Attitude Activation with a Pronunciation Task , 1996 .

[10]  L. Newman,et al.  Assimilation and Contrast Effects in Spontaneous Trait Inference , 1990 .

[11]  J. Bargh The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. , 1994 .

[12]  R. Fazio,et al.  Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  J. Bargh The automaticity of everyday life. , 1997 .

[14]  P. Devine Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. , 1989 .

[15]  J. Bargh,et al.  The Role of Consciousness in Priming Effects on Categorization , 1987 .

[16]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Preconscious Control of Stereotype Activation Through Chronic Egalitarian Goals , 1999 .

[17]  P. Herr,et al.  On the consequences of priming: Assimilation and contrast effects , 1983 .

[18]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  M. Ferguson,et al.  Beyond behaviorism: on the automaticity of higher mental processes. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  M. Zeelenberg,et al.  The impact of accuracy motivation on interpretation, comparison, and correction processes: accuracy x knowledge accessibility effects. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  B. Duncan,et al.  Differential social perception and attribution of intergroup violence: testing the lower limits of sterotyping of blacks. , 1976, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  Rainer Banse Affective priming with liked and disliked persons: Prime visibility determines congruency and incongruency effects , 2001 .

[23]  T. Chartrand,et al.  THE UNBEARABLE AUTOMATICITY OF BEING , 1999 .

[24]  M. J. Monteith,et al.  Self-regulation of prejudiced responses: Implications for progress in prejudice-reduction efforts. , 1993 .

[25]  Leonard L. Martin,et al.  Assimilation and contrast as a function of people's willingness and ability to expend effort in forming an impression. , 1990 .

[26]  E. Hilgard The trilogy of mind: cognition, affection, and conation. , 1980, Journal of the history of the behavioral sciences.

[27]  D. Dutton Tokenism, Reverse Discrimination, and Egalitarianism in Interracial Behavior , 1976 .

[28]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Automatic Activation of Impression Formation and Memorization Goals: Nonconscious Goal Priming Reproduces Effects of Explicit Task Instructions , 1996 .

[29]  K. Mogg,et al.  Processing of emotional information in anxious subjects. , 1990, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[30]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .

[31]  L. L. Martin,et al.  Set/reset: use and disuse of concepts in impression formation. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[32]  J. Kihlstrom The cognitive unconscious. , 1987, Science.

[33]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Effects of Epistemic Motivations on the Use of Accessible Constructs in Social Judgment , 1995 .

[34]  A G Greenwald,et al.  Unconscious processing of dichoptically masked words , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[35]  E. Higgins Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. , 1996 .

[36]  G. Logan Automaticity and cognitive control. , 1989 .

[37]  David M. Sanbonmatsu,et al.  On the automatic activation of attitudes. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[39]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Mental contamination and mental correction: unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[40]  Reinhard Pekrun,et al.  Directionality of affective priming: effects of trait anxiety and activation level. , 2003, Experimental psychology.

[41]  F. Strack,et al.  Awareness of the influence as a determinant of assimilation versus contrast , 1993 .

[42]  J. Kihlstrom The psychological unconscious. , 1990 .

[43]  Jeffrey W. Sherman,et al.  Suppression as a Stereotype Control Strategy , 1998, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[44]  G. Āllport The Nature of Prejudice , 1954 .

[45]  M. Banaji,et al.  Automatic and Controlled Processes in Stereotype Priming , 1996 .

[46]  W. S. Rholes,et al.  Category accessibility and impression formation , 1977 .

[47]  Richard L. Abrams,et al.  Three Cognitive Markers of Unconscious Semantic Activation , 1996, Science.

[48]  Shelly Chaiken,et al.  Accuracy motivation attenuates covert priming: The systematic reprocessing of social information. , 1994 .

[49]  J. Bargh,et al.  Automaticity in action: The unconscious as repository of chronic goals and motives. , 1996 .

[50]  J. Bargh,et al.  The automated will: nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[51]  C. I. Hovland,et al.  Social Judgment: Assimilation and Contrast Effects in Communication and Attitude Change , 1981 .