Domain-general involvement of the posterior frontolateral cortex in time-based resource-sharing in working memory: An fMRI study

Working memory is often defined in cognitive psychology as a system devoted to the simultaneous processing and maintenance of information. In line with the time-based resource-sharing model of working memory (TBRS; Barrouillet and Camos, 2015; Barrouillet et al., 2004), there is accumulating evidence that, when memory items have to be maintained while performing a concurrent activity, memory performance depends on the cognitive load of this activity, independently of the domain involved. The present study used fMRI to identify regions in the brain that are sensitive to variations in cognitive load in a domain-general way. More precisely, we aimed at identifying brain areas that activate during maintenance of memory items as a direct function of the cognitive load induced by both verbal and spatial concurrent tasks. Results show that the right IFJ and bilateral SPL/IPS are the only areas showing an increased involvement as cognitive load increases and do so in a domain general manner. When correlating the fMRI signal with the approximated cognitive load as defined by the TBRS model, it was shown that the main focus of the cognitive load-related activation is located in the right IFJ. The present findings indicate that the IFJ makes domain-general contributions to time-based resource-sharing in working memory and allowed us to generate the novel hypothesis by which the IFJ might be the neural basis for the process of rapid switching. We argue that the IFJ might be a crucial part of a central attentional bottleneck in the brain because of its inability to upload more than one task rule at once.

[1]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  Christopher L. Asplund,et al.  A Unified attentional bottleneck in the human brain , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  Christopher L. Asplund,et al.  Isolation of a Central Bottleneck of Information Processing with Time-Resolved fMRI , 2006, Neuron.

[4]  N. Cowan Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework , 1995 .

[5]  Myra A. Fernandes,et al.  Functional specificity of the visual word form area: General activation for words and symbols but specific network activation for words , 2008, Brain and Language.

[6]  F. Tong,et al.  Training Improves Multitasking Performance by Increasing the Speed of Information Processing in Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2009, Neuron.

[7]  Thomas A Hammeke,et al.  Neural basis of the Stroop interference task: Response competition or selective attention? , 2002, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[8]  R. H. Baayen,et al.  The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .

[9]  G J Hitch,et al.  Is there a Relationship between Task Demand and Storage Space in Tests of Working Memory Capacity? , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[10]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  Jan Derrfuss,et al.  Cognitive control in the posterior frontolateral cortex: evidence from common activations in task coordination, interference control, and working memory , 2004, NeuroImage.

[12]  M. Just,et al.  The neural bases of sentence comprehension: a fMRI examination of syntactic and lexical processing. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  R. Todd Constable,et al.  Similar and dissociable mechanisms for attention to internal versus external information , 2009, NeuroImage.

[14]  Mariko Osaka,et al.  Neural bases of focusing attention in working memory: An fMRI study based on group differences , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  M. Petrides,et al.  Dissociation within the Frontoparietal Network in Verbal Working Memory: A Parametric Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  P. Barrouillet,et al.  Time constraints and resource sharing in adults' working memory spans. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[17]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .

[18]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  A Parametric Study of Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Human Working Memory , 1996, NeuroImage.

[19]  B. Pfleiderer,et al.  Functional connectivity profile of the human inferior frontal junction: involvement in a cognitive control network , 2012, BMC Neuroscience.

[20]  Nelson Cowan,et al.  A Neural Region of Abstract Working Memory , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  Koji Jimura,et al.  On Verbal/Nonverbal Modality Dependence of Left and Right Inferior Prefrontal Activation during Performance of Flanker Interference Task , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural effect of stimulus-response modality compatibility on dual-task performance: an fMRI study , 2006, Psychological research.

[23]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[24]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of working memory: , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[25]  S Monsell,et al.  Naming the color of a word: Is it responses or task sets that compete? , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[26]  N Burgess,et al.  Recoding, storage, rehearsal and grouping in verbal short-term memory: an fMRI study , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  M. Chun,et al.  Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term memory for objects , 2006, Nature.

[28]  A. Miyake,et al.  Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control , 1999 .

[29]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  Theodore P. Zanto,et al.  Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual processing and working memory , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  P. Barrouillet,et al.  The impact of storage on processing: how is information maintained in working memory? , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[32]  P. Barrouillet,et al.  On the law relating processing to storage in working memory. , 2011, Psychological review.

[33]  Pierre Barrouillet,et al.  As Time Goes By , 2012 .

[34]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: A hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  Anjali Krishnan,et al.  Cluster-extent based thresholding in fMRI analyses: Pitfalls and recommendations , 2014, NeuroImage.

[36]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  Understanding serial position curves in short-term recognition and recall. , 2003 .

[37]  Anastasia Kiyonaga,et al.  The Working Memory Stroop Effect: When Internal Representations Clash With External Stimuli , 2014, Psychological science.

[38]  J. Jay Todd,et al.  Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex , 2004, Nature.

[39]  C. Frith,et al.  Neural Correlates of Attentional Capture in Visual Search , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Switching attention and resolving interference: fMRI measures of executive functions , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  Evie Vergauwe,et al.  Evidence for a central pool of general resources in working memory , 2012 .

[42]  Randall W Engle,et al.  Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[43]  M. Brass,et al.  Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: Meta‐analyses of switching and Stroop studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[44]  D. V. von Cramon,et al.  Localization of Executive Functions in Dual-Task Performance with fMRI , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  P. Barrouillet,et al.  Time and cognitive load in working memory. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[46]  P. Barrouillet,et al.  Visual and spatial working memory are not that dissociated after all: a time-based resource-sharing account. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[47]  M. Brass,et al.  The implementation of verbal instructions: An fMRI study , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[48]  Michael F. Bunting,et al.  Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[49]  Anastasia Kiyonaga,et al.  Resource-sharing between internal maintenance and external selection modulates attentional capture by working memory content , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[50]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  WordGen: A tool for word selection and nonword generation in Dutch, English, German, and French , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[51]  Evie Vergauwe,et al.  Do Mental Processes Share a Domain-General Resource? , 2010, Psychological science.

[52]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Selection for Cognitive Control: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study on the Selection of Task-Relevant Information , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  B. Gold,et al.  Domain general and domain preferential brain regions associated with different types of task switching: A Meta‐Analysis , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[54]  S2-1 Neural bases of focusing attention in working memory: an fMRI study based on individual differences , 2010, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[55]  M. Brass,et al.  The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[56]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  Top-down modulation of visual feature processing: The role of the inferior frontal junction , 2010, NeuroImage.

[57]  R. Frackowiak,et al.  Demonstrating the implicit processing of visually presented words and pseudowords. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[58]  Ben M. Crittenden,et al.  Task Difficulty Manipulation Reveals Multiple Demand Activity but no Frontal Lobe Hierarchy , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[59]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of interference in overlapping dual tasks: an fMRI study. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[60]  M. Brass,et al.  Decomposing Components of Task Preparation with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  Jason M. Chein,et al.  Domain-general mechanisms of complex working memory span , 2011, NeuroImage.

[62]  Valérie Camos,et al.  Working Memory: Loss and reconstruction , 2014 .

[63]  M. Brass,et al.  The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[64]  E. Crone,et al.  Neural evidence for dissociable components of task-switching. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[65]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[66]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Conjunction revisited , 2005, NeuroImage.

[67]  A. Dove,et al.  Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[68]  Angela R. Laird,et al.  Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate-based meta-analysis , 2012, NeuroImage.

[69]  N. Cowan An embedded-processes model of working memory , 1999 .