Initial directions and landing positions of binocular saccades

Existing models for the generation of saccades predict fixed trajectories between start and landing positions of saccades. Experimental data show that saccades have rather variable trajectories. The objective of the present research is to quantify the variability in trajectories of binocular saccades and to test in how far spatial variability can be described by adding noise to components of existing models. We studied the trajectories of self-paced saccades. Saccades were made between a number of stationary, visual targets lying in the frontal plane. More than 75 saccades were made to each target. Horizontal and vertical movements of both eyes were measured with a scleral coil technique. We defined the direction from starting position to end position of each primary saccade as the effective direction. We defined the direction from starting position to the eye position when the saccade had covered a distance of 2.5 deg as the initial direction of the saccade. We find that variability is two to seven times larger in initial directions than in effective directions. Effective directions are more accurate and more precise than initial directions. For each eye, initial and effective directions of saccades made to a particular target are negatively correlated, although in most cases rather weakly (0.1 < r2 < 0.5). Contrastingly, initial directions are always highly correlated (r2 > 0.8) of associated binocular saccades. High correlations are also found between effective directions. We conclude that curvedness of saccades is the result of a purposeful control strategy. The saccadic trajectories show that, initially, the eyes are accelerated roughly in the direction of the target and subsequently they are guided to the target. Analysis of possible models suggests that variability predominantly enters the saccadic system at a central stage of neuronal saccade generation. We conclude from simulations, in which we used different models of saccade generation, that the major sources of directional variability are part of a feedback loop. This conclusion provides indirect evidence for the presence of a feedback loop in the saccadic system.

[1]  J. T. Enright Changes in vergence mediated by saccades. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  L M Optican,et al.  Saccade-vergence interactions in humans. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  Casper J. Erkelens,et al.  The Initial Direction and Landing Position of Saccades , 1995 .

[4]  L. Stark,et al.  The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements , 1975 .

[5]  T. Vilis,et al.  The conjugacy of human saccadic eye movements , 1992, Vision Research.

[6]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Binocular co‐ordination of human vertical saccadic eye movements. , 1988, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[8]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Binocular co‐ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements. , 1988, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  L. Optican,et al.  Slow saccades in spinocerebellar degeneration. , 1976, Archives of neurology.

[10]  P. Bach-y-Rita,et al.  Basic Mechanisms of Ocular Motility and Their Clinical Implications , 1976 .

[11]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Precise recording of human eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[12]  H Collewijn,et al.  Ocular vergence under natural conditions. II. Gaze shifts between real targets differing in distance and direction , 1989, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B. Biological Sciences.

[13]  W. Becker,et al.  Human oblique saccades: Quantitative analysis of the relation between horizontal and vertical components , 1990, Vision Research.

[14]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Human gaze stability in the horizontal, vertical and torsional direction during voluntary head movements, evaluated with a three-dimensional scleral induction coil technique , 1987, Vision Research.

[15]  S. Gielen,et al.  A quantitative analysis of generation of saccadic eye movements by burst neurons. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  R. Yee,et al.  Velocities of vertical saccades with different eye movement recording methods. , 1985, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[17]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Oblique saccadic eye movements of primates. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Martin J. Steinbach,et al.  Nonadditivity of vergence and saccadic eye movement , 1978, Vision Research.

[19]  H. Collewijn,et al.  A direct test of Listing's law—II. Human ocular torsion measured under dynamic conditions , 1987, Vision Research.

[20]  Robert W. Kentridge,et al.  Eye movement research : mechanisms, processes and applications , 1995 .

[21]  J. T. Enright,et al.  Facilitation of vergence changes by saccades: influences of misfocused images and of disparity stimuli in man. , 1986, The Journal of physiology.

[22]  J. T. Enright The remarkable saccades of asymmetrical vergence , 1992, Vision Research.

[23]  C. Scudder A new local feedback model of the saccadic burst generator. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Hiroshi Ono,et al.  Changing fixation in the transverse plane at eye level and Hering's law of equal innervation , 1978, Vision Research.

[25]  Casper J. Erkelens,et al.  Voluntary binocular gaze-shifts in the plane of regard: Dynamics of version and vergence , 1995, Vision Research.