Classic EEG motor potentials track the emergence of value-based decisions

Making a value-based decision is a cognitively complex phenomenon and divisible into several sub-processes, such as the perception, evaluation, and final selection of choice options. Although previous research has attempted to dissociate these processes in the brain, there is emerging evidence that late action selection mechanisms are influenced continuously throughout the entire decision act. We used electroencephalography (EEG) and an established sequential decision making paradigm to investigate the extent to which the readiness potential (RP) and the lateralized readiness potential (LRP), two classic preparatory EEG motor components, reflect the ongoing evaluation process in value-based choices. During the task, human participants sequentially sampled probabilistic information to buy or reject offers of unknown value (using both hands) and were allowed to respond at any time. The pressure to respond was manipulated by charging low or high costs for collecting information. We modeled how and when decisions were made and found that participants adaptively lowered their threshold for required evidence with information costs and elapsed time. These shifts were accompanied by an increased RP-like signal during the decision process. The RP was further influenced by the amount of accumulated evidence. In addition, an LRP could be measured from the start of the decision process, well in advance and independent of the final decision. Our results are consistent with a continuous involvement of the brain's motor system in emerging value-based decisions and advocate using classic EEG motor potentials for studying neurocognitive theories of decision making.

[1]  F. Christian How the Brain Integrates Costs and Benefits During Decision Making , 2010 .

[2]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Building Bridges between Perceptual and Economic Decision-Making: Neural and Computational Mechanisms , 2012, Front. Neurosci..

[3]  James T. Townsend,et al.  The Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes , 1983 .

[4]  S. Bestmann,et al.  Time-Dependent Changes in Human Corticospinal Excitability Reveal Value-Based Competition for Action during Decision Processing , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Michael G. H. Coles,et al.  Mental chronometry and the study of human information processing. , 1995 .

[6]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[7]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[8]  James T. Townsend,et al.  Sequential sampling models of choice: Some recent advances , 2008 .

[9]  R. Marois,et al.  fMRI Evidence for a Dual Process Account of the Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff in Decision-Making , 2008, PloS one.

[10]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  Michael J. Spivey,et al.  Continuous Dynamics in Real-Time Cognition , 2006 .

[12]  J. Rieskamp,et al.  Deciding When to Decide: Time-Variant Sequential Sampling Models Explain the Emergence of Value-Based Decisions in the Human Brain , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  P. Cisek,et al.  Decisions in Changing Conditions: The Urgency-Gating Model , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  M. Hallett,et al.  Mesial motor areas in self-initiated versus externally triggered movements examined with fMRI: effect of movement type and rate. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Simon Farrell,et al.  Computational Modeling in Cognition: Principles and Practice , 2010 .

[16]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Decision-making with multiple alternatives , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  J. Ditterich,et al.  Neural Dynamics of Choice: Single-Trial Analysis of Decision-Related Activity in Parietal Cortex , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  John A. Nelder,et al.  A Simplex Method for Function Minimization , 1965, Comput. J..

[19]  M. K. van Vugt,et al.  EEG Oscillations Reveal Neural Correlates of Evidence Accumulation , 2012, Front. Neurosci..

[20]  L. Selen,et al.  Deliberation in the Motor System: Reflex Gains Track Evolving Evidence Leading to a Decision , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  B. Libet,et al.  Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  J. Ditterich Evidence for time‐variant decision making , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[23]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  The Neural and Computational Basis of Controlled Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff during Task Performance , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  G. Kreiman,et al.  Internally Generated Preactivation of Single Neurons in Human Medial Frontal Cortex Predicts Volition , 2011, Neuron.

[25]  Nick Chater,et al.  Salience driven value integration explains decision biases and preference reversal , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  A. Rangel,et al.  Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice. , 2010, Nature neuroscience.

[27]  E. Donchin,et al.  Detecting early communication: Using measures of movement-related potentials to illuminate human information processing , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[28]  M. Hallett,et al.  What is the Bereitschaftspotential? , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[29]  C. Padoa-Schioppa Neurobiology of economic choice: a good-based model. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.

[30]  Larissa Albantakis,et al.  A Multiple-Choice Task with Changes of Mind , 2012, PloS one.

[31]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Changing your mind: a computational mechanism of vacillation , 2009, Nature.

[32]  P. Haggard Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[33]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  An accumulator model for spontaneous neural activity prior to self-initiated movement , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  R. Bogacz,et al.  The neural basis of the speed–accuracy tradeoff , 2010, Trends in Neurosciences.

[35]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Striatum and pre-SMA facilitate decision-making under time pressure , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  David R. Cox,et al.  The Theory of Stochastic Processes , 1967, The Mathematical Gazette.

[37]  L. Deecke,et al.  The Preparation and Execution of Self-Initiated and Externally-Triggered Movement: A Study of Event-Related fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.

[38]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Temporal Integration of Olfactory Perceptual Evidence in Human Orbitofrontal Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[39]  J. Griffiths The Theory of Stochastic Processes , 1967 .

[40]  M. Coles Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[41]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[42]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Accumulation of Evidence during Sequential Decision Making: The Importance of Top–Down Factors , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The neural systems that mediate human perceptual decision making , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[44]  H. Kornhuber,et al.  Hirnpotentialänderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale , 1965, Pflüger's Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[45]  Stephan F Taylor,et al.  Updating Beliefs for a Decision: Neural Correlates of Uncertainty and Underconfidence , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  Probabilistic reasoning by neurons , 2007, Nature.

[47]  Andreas K. Engel,et al.  Buildup of Choice-Predictive Activity in Human Motor Cortex during Perceptual Decision Making , 2009, Current Biology.

[48]  S. Kelly,et al.  A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[50]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[51]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Modulation of activity in medial frontal and motor cortices during error observation , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  A. Nobre,et al.  Effects of Decision Variables and Intraparietal Stimulation on Sensorimotor Oscillatory Activity in the Human Brain , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  M. Posner Chronometric explorations of mind : the third Paul M. Fitts lectures, delivered at the University of Michigan, September 1976 , 1978 .

[54]  M. Brass,et al.  Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Transformation of stimulus value signals into motor commands during simple choice , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.