Sensitivity to Velocity- and Disparity-Based Cues to Motion-In-Depth With and Without Spared Stereopsis in Binocular Visual Impairment.

Purpose Two binocular sources of information serve motion-in-depth (MID) perception: changes in disparity over time (CD), and interocular velocity differences (IOVD). While CD requires the computation of small spatial disparities, IOVD could be computed from a much lower-resolution signal. IOVD signals therefore might still be available under conditions of binocular vision impairment (BVI) with limited or no stereopsis, for example, amblyopia. Methods Sensitivity to CD and IOVD was measured in adults who had undergone therapy to correct optical misalignment or amblyopia in childhood (n = 16), as well as normal vision controls with good stereoacuity (n = 8). Observers discriminated the interval containing a smoothly oscillating MID "test" stimulus from a "control" stimulus in a two-interval forced choice paradigm. Results Of the BVI observers with no static stereoacuity (n = 9), one displayed evidence for sensitivity to IOVD only, while there was otherwise no sensitivity for either CD or IOVD in the group. Generally, BVI observers with measurable stereoacuity (n = 7) displayed a pattern resembling the control group: showing a similar sensitivity for both cues. A neutral density filter placed in front of the fixing eye in a subset of BVI observers did not improve performance. Conclusions In one BVI observer there was preserved sensitivity to IOVD but not CD, though overall only those BVI observers with at least gross stereopsis were able to detect disparity- or velocity-based cues to MID. The results imply that these logically distinct information sources are somehow coupled, and in some cases BVI observers with no stereopsis may still retain sensitivity to IOVD.

[1]  B. G. Cumming,et al.  Binocular mechanisms for detecting motion-in-depth , 1994, Vision Research.

[2]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Physiological basis of anisometropic amblyopia. , 1978, Science.

[3]  K Toyama,et al.  Preservation of position and motion stereopsis in strabismic subjects. , 1987, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[4]  David Alais,et al.  Evidence for speed sensitivity to motion in depth from binocular cues. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[5]  R. Hess,et al.  Interocular suppression in normal and amblyopic vision: spatio-temporal properties. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[6]  Barry B. Lee,et al.  Suppressive Surrounds and Contrast Gain in Magnocellular-Pathway Retinal Ganglion Cells of Macaque , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  B. Julesz,et al.  Differences between monocular and binocular stroboscopic movement perception. , 1968, Vision research.

[8]  C Shawn Green,et al.  Optimal Combination of the Binocular Cues to 3D Motion. , 2015, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[9]  Benjamin Thompson,et al.  Binocular vision in amblyopia: structure, suppression and plasticity , 2014, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[10]  Yuichi Sakano,et al.  Aftereffect of motion-in-depth based on binocular cues: Effects of adaptation duration, interocular correlation, and temporal correlation. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[11]  Adam P. Morris,et al.  The (un)suitability of modern liquid crystal displays (LCDs) for vision research , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[12]  S. Wuerger,et al.  A Systematic Comparison of Static and Dynamic Cues for Depth Perception. , 2016, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[13]  Hirohisa Yaguchi,et al.  Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth. , 2008, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[14]  S. Dumoulin,et al.  Impaired Velocity Processing Reveals an Agnosia for Motion in Depth , 2016, Psychological science.

[15]  Jenny C. A. Read,et al.  Avoiding monocular artifacts in clinical stereotests presented on column-interleaved digital stereoscopic displays , 2016, Journal of vision.

[16]  Luke E. Hallum,et al.  Altered Balance of Receptive Field Excitation and Suppression in Visual Cortex of Amblyopic Macaque Monkeys , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Two Independent Mechanisms for Motion-In-Depth Perception: Evidence from Individual Differences , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[18]  Daniel H. Baker,et al.  Measurement of crosstalk in stereoscopic display systems used for vision research , 2016, Journal of vision.

[19]  R. Hess,et al.  Latent stereopsis for motion in depth in strabismic amblyopia. , 2009, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[20]  H Yaguchi,et al.  A new method for assessing motion-in-depth perception in strabismic patients , 2007, British Journal of Ophthalmology.

[21]  S. Coren,et al.  The dominant eye. , 1976, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  Bertram E. Shi,et al.  Neural population models for perception of motion in depth , 2014, Vision Research.

[23]  Lora T. Likova,et al.  Stereomotion processing in the human occipital cortex , 2007, NeuroImage.

[24]  Thaddeus B. Czuba,et al.  Speed and eccentricity tuning reveal a central role for the velocity-based cue to 3D visual motion. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  L. Cormack,et al.  Disparity- and velocity-based signals for three-dimensional motion perception in human MT+ , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  M L Braunstein,et al.  Stereoscopic Depth Perception by Static Stereo- Deficient Observers in Dynamic Displays with Constant and Changing Disparity , 1989, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[27]  Gregory C DeAngelis,et al.  Neural Representation of Motion-In-Depth in Area MT , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  Alex R. Wade,et al.  Evidence for an Optimal Algorithm Underlying Signal Combination in Human Visual Cortex , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  Rob Gray,et al.  Binocular processing of motion: some unresolved questions. , 2009, Spatial vision.

[30]  Alexander C Huk,et al.  Multiplexing in the primate motion pathway , 2012, Vision Research.

[31]  Bas Rokers,et al.  Strong percepts of motion through depth without strong percepts of position in depth. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[32]  W H Pratt Stereoscopic vision. , Science.

[33]  D. Hubel,et al.  Binocular interaction in striate cortex of kittens reared with artificial squint. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  Behzad Mansouri,et al.  Binocular summation of contrast remains intact in strabismic amblyopia. , 2007, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[35]  N. V. Swindale,et al.  Comparison of receptive field properties of neurons in area 17 of normal and bilaterally amblyopic cats , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Robert H. Black,et al.  Perceiving 3D in the absence of measurable stereo-acuity , 2014 .

[37]  J. Pokorny Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1972 .

[38]  Earl L. Smith,et al.  Early Monocular Defocus Disrupts the Normal Development of Receptive-Field Structure in V2 Neurons of Macaque Monkeys , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Thaddeus B. Czuba,et al.  Area MT Encodes Three-Dimensional Motion , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Hirohisa Yaguchi,et al.  Motion in depth based on inter-ocular velocity differences , 2000, Vision Research.

[41]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Seeing in Depth , 2008 .

[42]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[43]  Alexander M. Millkey The Black Swan: The Impact of the Highly Improbable , 2009 .

[44]  C. Tyler,et al.  Bayesian adaptive estimation of psychometric slope and threshold , 1999, Vision Research.

[45]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Binocular vision and motion-in-depth. , 2008, Spatial vision.

[46]  Thaddeus B. Czuba,et al.  Separate Perceptual and Neural Processing of Velocity- and Disparity-Based 3D Motion Signals , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  R. Hess,et al.  The amblyopic deficit and its relationship to geniculo-cortical processing streams. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Leland S Stone,et al.  Stereomotion speed perception: contributions from both changing disparity and interocular velocity difference over a range of relative disparities. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[49]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.