Perceptual and attentional influences on continuous 2:1 and 3:2 multi-frequency bimanual coordination.

Two experiments were conducted to determine if multi-frequency (2:1 and 3:2) coordination between the limbs is enhanced when integrated feedback is provided in the form of Lissajous plots, attention demands are reduced, and attempts to consciously coordinate the limbs are not encouraged. To determine the influence of vision of the limbs, covered and uncovered limb groups were provided online Lissajous feedback. To determine the impact of the Lissajous feedback, a control group that was not provided Lissajous feedback was also tested. The data indicated remarkably effective performances after 5 min of practice when limbs were covered and Lissajous feedback was provided. When Lissajous feedback was provided and vision of the limbs was permitted, performance deteriorated. Performance by the group not provided Lissajous feedback was quite poor. The findings suggest that some of the difficulty associated with producing difficult bimanual coordination patterns are due to the less than optimal perceptual information available in various testing situations and the attentional focus imposed by the participant.

[1]  Eric L. Amazeen,et al.  Visual–spatial and anatomical constraints interact in a bimanual coordination task with transformed visual feedback , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  S P Swinnen,et al.  Interaction of neuromuscular, spatial and visual constraints on hand-foot coordination dynamics. , 2005, Human movement science.

[3]  J. Kelso,et al.  Environmentally-specified patterns of movement coordination in normal and split-brain subjects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Relative Phase Alterations During Bimanual Skill Acquisition. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[5]  J. Kelso,et al.  Intentional switching between patterns of bimanual coordination depends on the intrinsic dynamics of the patterns. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[6]  G. Wulf,et al.  The learning advantages of an external focus of attention in golf. , 1999, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[7]  Hermann Haken,et al.  Neural and Synergetic Computers , 1988 .

[8]  J. Kelso,et al.  Action-Perception as a Pattern Formation Process , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.

[9]  G P Bingham,et al.  Visual perception of the relative phasing of human limb movements , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  M. Kinsbourne The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention. , 1970, Acta psychologica.

[11]  Sarah R Hurley,et al.  The influence of augmented feedback and prior learning on the acquisition of a new bimanual coordination pattern. , 2006, Human movement science.

[12]  M. Peters Do feedback processing, output variability, and spatial complexity account for manual asymmetries? , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[13]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  Multivariate time–frequency analysis of electromagnetic brain activity during bimanual motor learning , 2007, NeuroImage.

[14]  R. Ivry,et al.  Callosotomy patients exhibit temporal uncoupling during continuous bimanual movements , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  J. Kelso,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992 .

[16]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Performance asymmetries and coupling dynamics in the acquisition of multifrequency bimanual coordination , 1998 .

[17]  Stephan P Swinnen,et al.  AGE-RELATED DEFICITS IN MOTOR LEARNING AND DIFFERENCES IN FEEDBACK PROCESSING DURING THE PRODUCTION OF A BIMANUAL COORDINATION PATTERN. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[18]  G P Bingham,et al.  Visual perception of mean relative phase and phase variability. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  Winston D. Byblow,et al.  Performance asymmetries in multifrequency coordination , 1994 .

[20]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Dynamics of learning and transfer of muscular and spatial relative phase in bimanual coordination: evidence for abstract directional codes , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  C. Wright,et al.  Temporal rescaling of simple and complex ratios in rhythmic tapping. , 1995 .

[22]  P. Cavallari,et al.  Differential control of in-phase and anti-phase coupling of rhythmic movements of ipsilateral hand and foot , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  S. Swinnen Intermanual coordination: From behavioural principles to neural-network interactions , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[24]  M. Turvey,et al.  Handedness and the asymmetric dynamics of bimanual rhythmic coordination. , 1995 .

[25]  R. Barrett,et al.  Augmented visual feedback increases finger tremor during postural pointing , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  Y. Guiard On Fitts's and Hooke's laws: simple harmonic movement in upper-limb cyclical aiming. , 1993, Acta psychologica.

[27]  H. Haken,et al.  A stochastic theory of phase transitions in human hand movement , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[28]  Dagmar Sternad,et al.  Dynamics of 1:2 Coordination: Sources of Symmetry Breaking. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  G. Wulf,et al.  Extended Book Review: Attention and Motor Skill Learning , 2007 .

[30]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Resonance Constraints on Rhythmic Movement , 1993 .

[31]  J. Kelso,et al.  Multifrequency behavioral patterns and the phase attractive circle map , 1991, Biological Cybernetics.

[32]  John J Buchanan,et al.  One-to-One and Polyrhythmic Temporal Coordination in Bimanual Circle Tracing , 2006, Journal of motor behavior.

[33]  J. Kelso,et al.  Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations , 1986 .

[34]  David R. Collins,et al.  Perceptual coupling in rhythmic movement coordination: stable perception leads to stable action , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  P. V. Wieringen,et al.  Coupling strength in tapping a 2:3 polyrhythm , 1995 .

[36]  C. Shea,et al.  Increasing the distance of an external focus of attention enhances learning , 2003, Psychological research.

[37]  Stephan Riek,et al.  Hierarchical organisation of neuro-anatomical constraints in interlimb coordination. , 2005, Human movement science.

[38]  P J Beek,et al.  Movement-Related Sensory Feedback Mediates the Learning of a New Bimanual Relative Phase Pattern , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[39]  Paul Van Hecke,et al.  Internal vs external generation of movements: differential neural pathways involved in bimanual coordination performed in the presence or absence of augmented visual feedback , 2003, NeuroImage.

[40]  A. Opstal Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .

[41]  W. Prinz,et al.  Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination , 2001, Nature.

[42]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Attention and Handedness in Bimanual Coordination Dynamics , 1997 .

[43]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Bimanual 1:1 with 90° continuous relative phase: difficult or easy! , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  D Deutsch,et al.  The generation of two isochronous sequences in parallel , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  Paolo Cavallari,et al.  Preferential coupling between voluntary movements of ipsilateral limbs , 1982, Neuroscience Letters.

[46]  S. A. Wallace,et al.  Order parameters for the neural organization of single, multijoint limb movement patterns , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Unraveling interlimb interactions underlying bimanual coordination. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  A Perceptually Driven Dynamical Model of Bimanual Rhythmic Movement (and Phase Perception) , 2004 .

[49]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Using scanning trials to assess intrinsic coordination dynamics , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[50]  Pier-Giorgio Zanone,et al.  Attentional load associated with performing and stabilizing preferred bimanual patterns , 1999 .

[51]  J. Kelso,et al.  Governing coordination: behavioural principles and neural correlates , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Amplitude differences, spatial assimilation, and integrated feedback in bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  A model for phase transitions in human hand movements during multifrequency tapping , 1996 .

[54]  G Aschersleben,et al.  Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[55]  S P Swinnen,et al.  Component variability during bimanual rhythmic movements: not all harmonic timing ratios are alike. , 1998, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[56]  Nicole Wenderoth,et al.  Changes in Brain Activation during the Acquisition of a Multifrequency Bimanual Coordination Task: From the Cognitive Stage to Advanced Levels of Automaticity , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  Dagmar Sternad,et al.  Dynamics of 1:2 Coordination: Generalizing Relative Phase to n:m Rhythms. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[58]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  Chapter 18 Another timing variable composed of state variables: Phase perception and phase driven oscillators , 2004 .

[59]  Journal de Psychologie, Normale et Pathologique , 1909 .

[60]  Olivier Oullier,et al.  Plane of Motion Mediates the Coalition of Constraints in Rhythmic Bimanual Coordination , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[61]  D J Povel,et al.  Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[62]  John J Buchanan,et al.  The Interaction of Tactile Information and Movement Amplitude in a Multijoint Bimanual Circle-Tracing Task: Phase Transitions and Loss of Stability , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[63]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Two hands, one brain: cognitive neuroscience of bimanual skill , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[64]  A. J. Kovacs,et al.  Perceptual influences on Fitts’ law , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[65]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Impossible is nothing: 5:3 and 4:3 multi-frequency bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[66]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Laterally focused attention modulates asymmetric coupling in rhythmic interlimb coordination , 2008, Psychological research.

[67]  J. Kelso Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.

[68]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Two coupled oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping by both hands , 1980, Biological Cybernetics.

[69]  J. Summers,et al.  Attentional costs of coordinating homologous and non-homologous limbs. , 2004, Human movement science.

[70]  Andrew D. Wilson,et al.  Human movement coordination implicates relative direction as the information for relative phase , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[71]  R J Jagacinski,et al.  Can people tap concurrent bimanual rhythms independently? , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.

[72]  G. Wulf,et al.  An External Focus of Attention Enhances Golf Shot Accuracy in Beginners and Experts , 2007, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[73]  H. Haken,et al.  A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[74]  J A Kelso,et al.  Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. , 1988, Science.

[75]  Vladimir Igorevich Arnold,et al.  Geometrical Methods in the Theory of Ordinary Differential Equations , 1983 .

[76]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Interlimb coupling strength scales with movement amplitude , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[77]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Error correction in bimanual coordination benefits from bilateral muscle activity: evidence from kinesthetic tracking , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[78]  Paul Van Hecke,et al.  Brain Areas Involved in Interlimb Coordination: A Distributed Network , 2001, NeuroImage.

[79]  G. Wulf,et al.  Enhancing the Learning of Sport Skills Through External-Focus Feedback , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[80]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Part and whole perceptual-motor practice of a polyrhythm , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[81]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Interlimb coordination: Learning and transfer under different feedback conditions , 1997 .

[82]  J. S. Scott Kelso,et al.  Haptic information stabilizes and destabilizes coordination dynamics , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[83]  Andras Semjen,et al.  Instabilities during antiphase bimanual movements: are ipsilateral pathways involved? , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[84]  John J. Buchanan,et al.  Learning an environment–actor coordination skill: visuomotor transformation and coherency of perceptual structure , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[85]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Multifrequency coordination in bimanual tapping: Asymmetrical coupling and signs of supercriticality. , 1995 .

[86]  J. Summers,et al.  The acquisition of bimanual coordination is mediated by anisotropic coupling between the hands. , 2002, Human movement science.

[87]  M. Peters,et al.  Coordination of the two hands and effects of attentional manipulation in the production of a bimanual 2:3 polyrhythm , 1989 .

[88]  S. Bogacz Understanding How Speed Affects Performance of Polyrhythms: Transferring Control as Speed Increases , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[89]  J. Kelso,et al.  Posturally induced transitions in rhythmic multijoint limb movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[90]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Relative phase destabilization during interlimb coordination: the disruptive role of kinesthetic afferences induced by passive movement , 1990, Experimental Brain Research.