Observational Learning: Tell Beginners What They Are about to Watch and They Will Learn Better

Observation aids motor skill learning. When multiple models or different levels of performance are observed, does learning improve when the observer is informed of the performance quality prior to each observation trial or after each trial? We used a knock-down barrier task and asked participants to learn a new relative timing pattern that differed from that naturally emerging from the task constraints (Blandin et al., 1999). Following a physical execution pre-test, the participants observed two models demonstrating different levels of performance and were either informed of this performance prior to or after each observation trial. The results of the physical execution retention tests of the two experiments reported in the present study indicated that informing the observers of the demonstration quality they were about to see aided learning more than when this information was provided after each observation trial. Our results suggest that providing advanced information concerning the quality of the observation may help participants detect errors in the model's performance, which is something that novice participants have difficulty doing, and then learn from these observations.

[1]  J Zuckerman,et al.  Modeling effects on motor performance. , 1976, Research quarterly.

[2]  Salvatore Maria Aglioti,et al.  Somatotopic mapping of piano fingering errors in sensorimotor experts: TMS studies in pianists and visually trained musically naives. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  Paola Cesari,et al.  Fooling the kickers but not the goalkeepers: behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of fake action detection in soccer. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  A. Badets,et al.  Observational Learning: Effects of Bandwidth Knowledge of Results , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[5]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  C. Urgesi,et al.  Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  L Proteau,et al.  On the cognitive processes underlying contextual interference and observational learning. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[8]  B J Pollock,et al.  Effects of the model's skill level on observational motor learning. , 1992, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[9]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Premotor Cortex Activation during Observation and Naming of Familiar Tools , 1997, NeuroImage.

[10]  P McCullagh,et al.  Learning versus correct models: influence of model type on the learning of a free-weight squat lift. , 1997, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[11]  Young U. Ryu,et al.  Observational practice of relative but not absolute motion features in a single-limb multi-joint coordination task , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  Romeo Chua,et al.  The Role of Video in Facilitating Perception and Action of a Novel Coordination Movement , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.

[13]  Juliana Dushanova,et al.  Neurons in primary motor cortex engaged during action observation , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[14]  D L Wright,et al.  Cognitive processes related to contextual interference and observational learning: a replication of Blandin, Proteau and Alain (1994). , 1997, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[15]  A Bandura,et al.  Representational guidance of action production in observational learning: a causal analysis. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Evidence of Mirror Neurons in Human Inferior Frontal Gyrus , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  John F. Kalaska,et al.  Neural correlates of mental rehearsal in dorsal premotor cortex , 2004, Nature.

[18]  V. Gallese Action representaion and the inferior parietal lobule , 2000 .

[19]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror mechanism: recent findings and perspectives , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  C. Wright,et al.  Temporal rescaling of sample and complex rations in rhythmic tapping. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Luciano Fadiga,et al.  Effect of weight-related labels on corticospinal excitability during observation of grasping: a TMS study , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Influence of an unskilled model's practice schedule on observational motor learning , 1990 .

[23]  Giacomo Koch,et al.  The role of mirror neurons in observational motor learning: an integrative review , 2014 .

[24]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[25]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[26]  Arnaud Badets,et al.  The Role of Knowledge of Results Frequency in Learning Through Observation , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[27]  C. Heyes,et al.  Motor learning by observation: Evidence from a serial reaction time task , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[28]  Luc Proteau,et al.  Learning through observation: a combination of expert and novice models favors learning , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  L. Proteau,et al.  Cognitive Processes Underlying Observational Learning of Motor Skills , 1999 .

[30]  Luc Proteau,et al.  Observation learning of a motor task: who and when? , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  F. Samejima Cognitive processes. , 2021, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[32]  R. Cañal-Bruland,et al.  Foul or dive? Motor contributions to judging ambiguous foul situations in football , 2014, European journal of sport science.

[33]  Carol Leone Cross The role of knowledge of results in developing an error detection mechanism when learning an open loop movement task , 1997 .

[34]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Observing how others lift light or heavy objects: Which visual cues mediate the encoding of muscular force in the primary motor cortex? , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  John J Buchanan,et al.  Specificity in practice benefits learning in novice models and variability in demonstration benefits observational practice , 2010, Psychological research.

[36]  Simon J. Bennett,et al.  General motor representations are developed during action-observation , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[38]  E Macaluso,et al.  Action anticipation beyond the action observation network: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in expert basketball players , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[39]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Cortical mechanisms underlying the organization of goal-directed actions and mirror neuron-based action understanding. , 2014, Physiological reviews.

[40]  M. Weiss,et al.  Modeling Considerations in Motor Skill Acquisition and Performance: An Integrated, Approach , 1989, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[41]  S. Vogt,et al.  From visuo-motor interactions to imitation learning: Behavioural and brain imaging studies , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.

[42]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Effector-dependent learning by observation of a finger movement sequence. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  Luc Proteau,et al.  Mixed observation favors motor learning through better estimation of the model’s performance , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  Alison J. Wiggett,et al.  Surface-Based Information Mapping Reveals Crossmodal Vision–Action Representations in Human Parietal and Occipitotemporal Cortex , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Cognitive Effort and Motor Learning , 1994 .

[46]  David L Wright,et al.  Error Detection Processes During Observational Learning , 2006, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[47]  E. Procyk,et al.  Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  Luciano Fadiga,et al.  Force requirements of observed object lifting are encoded by the observer’s motor system: a TMS study , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[49]  Michael I. Jordan Computational aspects of motor control and motor learning , 2008 .

[50]  Remo Guidieri Res , 1995, RES: Anthropology and Aesthetics.

[51]  Simon J. Bennett,et al.  Specificity of Task Constraints and Effects of Visual Demonstrations and Verbal Instructions in Directing Learners' Search During Skill Acquisition , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[52]  Bruce Abernethy,et al.  Functional MRI reveals expert-novice differences during sport-related anticipation , 2010, Neuroreport.

[53]  Rudolf Stark,et al.  The influence of expertise on brain activation of the action observation network during anticipation of tennis and volleyball serves , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[54]  Diane M. Ste-Marie,et al.  Observation interventions for motor skill learning and performance: an applied model for the use of observation , 2012 .

[55]  D L Wright,et al.  Can Observational Practice Facilitate Error Recognition and Movement Production? , 2000, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[56]  A. Williams,et al.  What is modelled during observational learning? , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.