Reversed short-latency ocular following
暂无分享,去创建一个
F. A. Miles | F. Miles | G. Masson | D.-S Yang | G. S Masson | F. A Miles | D-S Yang
[1] George Sperling,et al. 1st- and 2nd-order motion and texture resolution in central and peripheral vision , 1995, Vision Research.
[2] S. M. Axstis. PHI MOVEMENT AS A SUBTRACTION PROCESS , 1970 .
[3] Lance M. Optican,et al. Unix-based multiple-process system, for real-time data acquisition and control , 1982 .
[4] Vision Research , 1961, Nature.
[5] Ning Qian,et al. Computing Stereo Disparity and Motion with Known Binocular Cell Properties , 1994, Neural Computation.
[6] F A Miles,et al. Ocular responses to translation and their dependence on viewing distance. II. Motion of the scene. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.
[7] F A Miles,et al. The neural processing of 3‐D visual information: evidence from eye movements , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.
[8] David J. Fleet,et al. Neural encoding of binocular disparity: Energy models, position shifts and phase shifts , 1996, Vision Research.
[9] ANDREW T SMITH,et al. Separate Detection of Moving Luminance and Contrast Modulations: Fact or Artifact? , 1997, Vision Research.
[10] S. Anstis,et al. Illusory reversal of visual depth and movement during changes of contrast , 1975, Vision Research.
[11] P J Benson,et al. Stages in motion processing revealed by the ocular following response. , 1999, Neuroreport.
[12] Robert Fox,et al. Stereoscopic contours induce optokinetic nystagmus , 1978, Vision Research.
[13] A. T. Smith,et al. Motion detection in human vision: a unifying approach based on energy and features , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[14] B. G. Cumming,et al. Responses of primary visual cortical neurons to binocular disparity without depth perception , 1997, Nature.
[15] C. Busettini,et al. A role for stereoscopic depth cues in the rapid visual stabilization of the eyes , 1996, Nature.
[16] U. Ilg,et al. Initiation of smooth-pursuit eye movements to first-order and second-order motion stimuli , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] M. Landy,et al. Kinetic depth effect and optic flow—I. 3D shape from Fourier motion , 1989, Vision Research.
[18] I. Ohzawa,et al. Stereoscopic depth discrimination in the visual cortex: neurons ideally suited as disparity detectors. , 1990, Science.
[19] C. Busettini,et al. Human ocular responses to translation of the observer and of the scene: dependence on viewing distance , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] Z L Lu,et al. Three-systems theory of human visual motion perception: review and update. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[21] H. Collewijn,et al. Precise recording of human eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.
[22] F. A. Miles,et al. The role of MST neurons during ocular tracking in 3D space. , 2000, International review of neurobiology.
[23] F. A. Miles,et al. Vergence eye movements in response to binocular disparity without depth perception , 1997, Nature.
[24] A. Gorea. Spatiotemporal characterization of a Fourier and non-Fourier motion system , 1995, Vision Research.
[25] C Busettini,et al. Short-latency disparity vergence in humans. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[26] F. A. Miles,et al. Single-unit activity in cortical area MST associated with disparity-vergence eye movements: evidence for population coding. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[27] E. Castet,et al. Temporal dynamics of motion integration for the initiation of tracking eye movements at ultra-short latencies , 2000, Visual Neuroscience.
[28] A. Fuchs,et al. A method for measuring horizontal and vertical eye movement chronically in the monkey. , 1966, Journal of applied physiology.
[29] Steven M. Archer,et al. Stereoscopic contours and optokinetic nystagmus in normal and stereoblind subjects , 1987, Vision Research.
[30] F A Miles,et al. Short-latency disparity vergence responses and their dependence on a prior saccadic eye movement. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[31] Takao Sato,et al. Reversed apparent motion with random dot patterns , 1989, Vision Research.
[32] Margaret S Livingstone,et al. Two-Dimensional Substructure of MT Receptive Fields , 2001, Neuron.
[33] Laurence R. Harris,et al. Interactions between first- and second-order motion revealed by optokinetic nystagmus , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[34] G Sperling,et al. Two motion perception mechanisms revealed through distance-driven reversal of apparent motion. , 1989, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[35] G Sperling,et al. Second-order reversed phi , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[36] A. T. Smith,et al. Motion defined exclusively by second-order characteristics does not evoke optokinetic nystagmus , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.
[37] F. A. Miles,et al. Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. I. Dependence on temporospatial properties of visual input. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.
[38] F A Miles,et al. Short latency ocular-following responses in man , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.
[39] S. Nishida. Spatiotemporal properties of motion perception for random-check contrast modulations , 1993, Vision Research.
[40] F A Miles,et al. Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. II. Dependence on a prior saccadic eye movement. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.
[41] L. Spillmann,et al. Reversed Visual Motion and Self-Sustaining Eye Oscillations , 1997, Perception.
[42] C Busettini,et al. Short-latency ocular following in humans: sensitivity to binocular disparity , 2001, Vision Research.
[43] A. Derrington,et al. Discriminating the direction of second-order motion at short stimulus durations , 1993, Vision Research.
[44] Johannes M. Zanker,et al. Smooth-pursuit eye movements elicited by first-order and second-order motion , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.