Foraging currencies, metabolism and behavioural routines.

A fundamental issue in foraging theory is whether it is possible to find a simple currency that characterizes foraging behaviour. If such a currency exists, then it is tempting to argue that the selective forces that have shaped the evolution of foraging behaviour have been understood. We review previous work on currencies for the foraging behaviour of an animal that maximizes total energy gained. In many circumstances, it is optimal to maximize a suitably modified form of efficiency. We show how energy gain, predation and damage can be combined in a single currency based on reproductive value. We draw attention to the idea that hard work may have an adverse effect on an animal's condition. We develop a model of optimal foraging over a day when a forager's state consists of its energy reserves and its condition. Optimal foraging behaviour in our model depends on energy reserves, condition and time of day. The pattern of optimal behaviour depends strongly on assumptions about the probability that the forager is killed by a predator. If condition is important, no simple currency characterizes foraging behaviour, but behaviour can be understood in terms of the maximization of reproductive value. It may be optimal to adopt a foraging option that results in a rate of energy expenditure that is less than the rate associated with maximizing efficiency.

[1]  S. Fretwell,et al.  On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds , 1969 .

[2]  V. Savage,et al.  Temperature dependence of trophic interactions are driven by asymmetry of species responses and foraging strategy. , 2014, The Journal of animal ecology.

[3]  Pete C. Trimmer,et al.  Generalized Optimal Risk Allocation: Foraging and Antipredator Behavior in a Fluctuating Environment , 2012, The American Naturalist.

[4]  A. Houston,et al.  Ideal free distributions under predation risk , 1996, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[5]  A. Kacelnik CENTRAL PLACE FORAGING IN STARLINGS (STURNUS-VULGARIS) .1. PATCH RESIDENCE TIME , 1984 .

[6]  A. Houston,et al.  Optimal annual routines: behaviour in the context of physiology and ecology , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  H. Hirakawa Digestion-constrained optimal foraging in generalist mammalian herbivores , 1997 .

[8]  H. Hirakawa Diet optimization with a nutrient or toxin constraint. , 1995, Theoretical population biology.

[9]  A. Houston Models of optimal avian migration: state time and predation , 1998 .

[10]  J. Jeschke When carnivores are “full and lazy” , 2007, Oecologia.

[11]  A. Houston,et al.  What Currency Do Bumble Bees Maximize? , 2010, PloS one.

[12]  R. Drent,et al.  The Prudent Parent: Energetic Adjustments in Avian Breeding 1) , 1980 .

[13]  J. Diamond,et al.  Maximal sustained energy budgets in humans and animals , 1997, Nature.

[14]  A. Houston Optimal foraging by parent birds feeding dependent young , 1987 .

[15]  G. Belovsky,et al.  Diet optimization in a generalist herbivore: the moose. , 1978, Theoretical population biology.

[16]  A. Houston,et al.  Patch choice and population size , 1997, Evolutionary Ecology.

[17]  A. Houston,et al.  Central-place foraging in honey bees: the effect of travel time and nectar flow on crop filling , 1986, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[18]  A. Houston,et al.  The effect of thermoregulatory substitution on optimal energy reserves of small birds in winter , 2004 .

[19]  Alasdair I. Houston,et al.  A theoretical investigation of the fat reserves and mortality levels of small birds in winter , 1993 .

[20]  W. Sutherland,et al.  The Inactivity of Animals: Influence of Stochasticity and Prey Size , 1985 .

[21]  David Vasseur,et al.  Competition and the density dependence of metabolic rates. , 2014, The Journal of animal ecology.

[22]  Theunis Piersma,et al.  Digestive bottleneck affects foraging decisions in red knots Calidris canutus. I. Prey choice , 2005 .

[23]  O. Schmitz,et al.  Reconciling variability and optimal behaviour using multiple criteria in optimization models , 2004, Evolutionary Ecology.

[24]  A I Houston,et al.  Currencies For Foraging Based on Energetic Gain , 1997, The American Naturalist.

[25]  A. Houston,et al.  Honeybees maximize efficiency by not filling their crop , 1985, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[26]  J. Herbers Time resources and laziness in animals , 1981, Oecologia.

[27]  S. L. Lima,et al.  Foraging routines of small birds in winter: a theoretical investigation , 1994 .

[28]  A. Houston,et al.  State-dependent life histories , 1996, Nature.

[29]  Alasdair I. Houston,et al.  The value of food: effects of open and closed economies , 1989, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  A. Houston,et al.  Optimal moult strategies in migratory birds , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[31]  Willem Bouten,et al.  Pareto front analysis of flight time and energy use in long-distance bird migration , 2007 .

[32]  Gerry C. Emmans,et al.  Optimization of short-term animal behaviour and the currency of time , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[33]  S. Verhulst,et al.  Optimal foraging on perilous prey: risk of bill damage reduces optimal prey size in oystercatchers , 2006 .

[34]  A. Houston,et al.  The Common Currency for Behavioral Decisions , 1986, The American Naturalist.

[35]  John M. Fryxell,et al.  THE TEMPORAL SCALE OF FORAGING DECISIONS IN BISON , 2002 .

[36]  B. Ames,et al.  The free radical theory of aging matures. , 1998, Physiological reviews.

[37]  M. Kersten The rate of food processing in the Oystercatcher: Food intake and energy expenditure constrained by a digestive bottleneck , 1996 .

[38]  T. Schoener Theory of Feeding Strategies , 1971 .

[39]  A. Houston,et al.  A theoretical investigation of the effect of predators on foraging behaviour and energy reserves , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  A. Houston,et al.  Deterioration, death and the evolution of reproductive restraint in late life , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[41]  M. Mesterton-Gibbons On compromise in foraging and an experiment by Krebs et al. (1977) , 1989 .

[42]  N. Metcalfe,et al.  Oxidative stress as a mediator of life history trade-offs: mechanisms, measurements and interpretation. , 2009, Ecology letters.

[43]  R. S. Sohal,et al.  Mechanisms of aging: an appraisal of the oxidative stress hypothesis. , 2002, Free radical biology & medicine.

[44]  A. Hedenström,et al.  OPTIMAL FLIGHT SPEED OF BIRDS , 1995 .

[45]  J. Lucas,et al.  The costs of being cool: a dynamic model of nocturnal hypothermia by small food‐caching birds in winter , 2000 .

[46]  B. Nolet Efficiency as a foraging currency in animals attaining a gain below the energetic ceiling , 2002 .

[47]  E. Werner,et al.  THE ONTOGENETIC NICHE AND SPECIES INTERACTIONS IN SIZE-STRUCTURED POPULATIONS , 1984 .

[48]  J. Speakman,et al.  High hunting costs make African wild dogs vulnerable to kleptoparasitism by hyaenas , 1998, Nature.

[49]  J. McNamara,et al.  Stress, resource allocation, and mortality , 2005 .

[50]  R. Holt,et al.  A metabolic perspective on competition and body size reductions with warming. , 2014, The Journal of animal ecology.

[51]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  Time and energy constraints and the relationships between currencies in foraging theory , 1994 .

[52]  A. Hedenström,et al.  Optimum fuel loads in migratory birds: distinguishing between time and energy minimization , 1997, Journal of theoretical biology.

[53]  M. Cichoń,et al.  Diversity of age‐specific reproductive rates may result from ageing and optimal resource allocation , 2001, Journal of evolutionary biology.

[54]  A. Houston Energetic constraints and foraging efficiency , 1995 .

[55]  Alasdair I. Houston,et al.  Risk-sensitive foraging: A review of the theory , 1992 .

[56]  A. Houston,et al.  A Dynamic Model of Hypothermia as an Adaptive Response by Small Birds to Winter Conditions , 2002, Acta biotheoretica.

[57]  J. F. Gilliam,et al.  Habitat Selection Under Predation Hazard: Test of a Model with Foraging Minnows. , 1987, Ecology.

[58]  A. Houston,et al.  The use of a time and energy budget model of a parent bird to investigate limits to fledging mass in the Thick-billed Murre , 1996 .

[59]  A. Houston Evolutionary models of metabolism, behaviour and personality , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[60]  A. Houston,et al.  Annual routines of non‐migratory birds: optimal moult strategies , 2006 .

[61]  Alasdair I. Houston,et al.  The influence of mortality on the behaviour that maximizes reproductive success in a patchy environment , 1986 .

[62]  Jonathan M Yearsley,et al.  A life history model of somatic damage associated with resource acquisition: damage protection or prevention? , 2005, Journal of theoretical biology.

[63]  J. V. van Gils,et al.  Diet selection in a molluscivore shorebird across Western Europe: does it show short- or long-term intake rate-maximization? , 2010, The Journal of animal ecology.

[64]  A. Houston,et al.  State-dependent ideal free distributions , 1990, Evolutionary Ecology.