Visuomotor integration deficits precede clinical onset in Huntington's disease

OBJECTIVES Visuomotor integration deficits have been documented in Huntington disease (HD), with disproportionately more impairment when direct visual feedback is unavailable. Visuomotor integration under direct and indirect visual feedback conditions has not been investigated in the stage before clinical onset ('premanifest'). However, given evidence of posterior cortical atrophy in premanifest HD, we predicted visuomotor integration would be adversely affected, with greater impairment under conditions of indirect visual feedback. METHODS 239 subjects with the HD CAG expansion, ranging from more than a decade before predicted clinical onset until early stage disease, and 122 controls, completed a circle-tracing task, which included both direct and indirect visual feedback conditions. Measures included accuracy, speed, and speed of error detection and correction. Using brain images acquired with 3T magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), we generated grey and white matter volumes with voxel-based morphometry, and analyzed correlations with circle-tracing performance. RESULTS Compared with controls, early HD was associated with lower accuracy and slower performance in both circle-tracing conditions. Premanifest HD was associated with lower accuracy in both conditions and fewer rotations in the direct condition. Comparing performance in the indirect condition with the direct condition, HD gene expansion-carriers exhibited a disproportionate increase in errors relative to controls. Premanifest and early HD groups required longer to detect and correct errors, especially in the indirect condition. Slower performance in the indirect condition was associated with lower grey matter volumes in the left somatosensory cortex in VBM analyses. CONCLUSIONS Visuomotor integration deficits are evident many years before the clinical onset of HD, with deficits in speed, accuracy, and speed of error detection and correction. The visuomotor transformation demands of the indirect condition result in a disproportionate decrease in accuracy in the HD groups. Slower performance under indirect visual feedback was associated with atrophy of the left-hemisphere somatosensory cortex, which may reflect the proprioceptive demands of the task.

[1]  H Burton,et al.  Attending to and Remembering Tactile Stimuli: A Review of Brain Imaging Data and Single-Neuron Responses , 2000, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[2]  S. Fahn,et al.  Huntington disease , 1979, Neurology.

[3]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Motor disorder in Huntington's disease begins as a dysfunction in error feedback control , 2000, Nature.

[4]  A. Gordon,et al.  Altered movement trajectories and force control during object transport in Huntington's disease , 2001, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[5]  C. Marsden,et al.  The functions of the basal ganglia and the paradox of stereotaxic surgery in Parkinson's disease. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  F. Richer,et al.  Early Huntington’s disease affects movements in transformed sensorimotor mappings , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[7]  S. Zeger,et al.  Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models , 1986 .

[8]  M. MacDonald,et al.  CAG repeat number governs the development rate of pathology in Huntington's disease , 1997, Annals of neurology.

[9]  W. McIlroy,et al.  Tactile stimulus predictability modulates activity in a tactile-motor cortical network , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  H. Freund,et al.  Impairment of rapid movement in Huntington's disease. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  A. Dale,et al.  Regional and progressive thinning of the cortical ribbon in Huntington’s disease , 2002, Neurology.

[12]  A. Albanese,et al.  A study of arm movements in Huntington's disease under visually controlled and blindfolded conditions , 2003, Neurological Sciences.

[13]  J. Bradshaw,et al.  Reliance on advance information and movement sequencing in huntington's disease , 1995, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[14]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  A new model for prediction of the age of onset and penetrance for Huntington's disease based on CAG length , 2004, Clinical genetics.

[15]  K. Willmes,et al.  Huntington's disease: visuomotor disturbance in patients and offspring. , 1985, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[16]  F. Richer,et al.  Huntington's disease affects movement termination , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[17]  Nick C Fox,et al.  Biological and clinical manifestations of Huntington's disease in the longitudinal TRACK-HD study: cross-sectional analysis of baseline data , 2009, The Lancet Neurology.

[18]  A. Beuter,et al.  Sensorimotor mapping affects movement correction deficits in early Huntington’s disease , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Action monitoring, error detection, and the basal ganglia: an ERP study , 2001, Neuroreport.

[20]  B Fischl,et al.  Regional cortical thinning in preclinical Huntington disease and its relationship to cognition , 2005, Neurology.