Perseveration in behavioral neurology

We tested a new taxonomy of perseverative behavior consisting of three categories: (1) repetition of a previous response to a subsequent stimulus (recurrent), (2) inappropriate maintenance of a category of activity (stuck-in-set), and (3) abnormal prolongation of a current activity (continuous). Three groups of neurologically impaired subjects (with aphasia, right hemisphere damage, and Parkinson's disease) were administered tests to elicit each category of perseveration. Patients with aphasia produced significantly more recurrent peneveration than did patients with right hemisphere damage or healthy controls. Stuck-in-set perseveration was associated with dopamine system dysfunction, and continuous perseveration with right hemisphere damage. We propose a theory of perseveration dependent on anatomic, neuropsychological, and pharmacologic factors related to cerebral dominance. According to this theory, disruption of specific anatomic and pharmacologic systems produces different forms of perseveration which, in turn, underlie particular neurobehavioral disorders.

[1]  E. A. Berg,et al.  A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. , 1948, The Journal of general psychology.

[2]  K. H. Pribram,et al.  Language and Language Disturbances , 1949, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[3]  R. Schwab,et al.  Performance with multiple sets in Parkinson's disease☆ , 1964 .

[4]  A. Luria,et al.  TWO KINDS OF MOTOR PERSEVERATION IN MASSIVE INJURY OF THE FRONTAL LOBES. , 1965, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  T. Freeman,et al.  Perseveration—The Clinical Symptoms—In Chronic Schizophrenia and Organic Dementia , 1966, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[6]  L. Hurwitz,et al.  On perseveration in aphasics. , 1967, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  C. Butter,et al.  Perseveration of responding and nonresponding in monkeys with orbital frontal ablations. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[8]  W. Nauta The problem of the frontal lobe: a reinterpretation. , 1971, Journal of psychiatric research.

[9]  R. Schvaneveldt,et al.  Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  C. Adcock,et al.  Primary Mental Abilities. , 1971, The Journal of general psychology.

[11]  J. Scheel-Krüger,et al.  The effect of dopamine and noradrenaline antagonists on amphetamine induced locomotor activity in mice and rats. , 2009, Acta pharmacologica et toxicologica.

[12]  D. Drachman,et al.  Human memory and the cholinergic system. A relationship to aging? , 1974, Archives of neurology.

[13]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[14]  Joseph W. Helmick,et al.  Perseveration in brain-injured adults. , 1976, Journal of communication disorders.

[15]  Brenda Milner,et al.  Design fluency: The invention of nonsense drawings after focal cortical lesions , 1977, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Don L. Scarborough,et al.  Frequency and Repetition Effects in Lexical Memory. , 1977 .

[17]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Cognitive deficit caused by regional depletion of dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey. , 1979, Science.

[18]  E. Goldberg,et al.  Motor perseveration and long-term memory for visual forms , 1979 .

[19]  H. Whitaker,et al.  On Linguistic Perseveration , 1979 .

[20]  S. T. Mason,et al.  Theories of the dorsal bundle extinction effect , 1979, Brain Research Reviews.

[21]  H. Anisman,et al.  Amphetamine models of paranoid schizophrenia: an overview and elaboration of animal experimentation. , 1980, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  A. Yamadori Verbal perseveration in aphasia , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  M. Mesulam A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.

[24]  S. Blumstein,et al.  Lexical decision and aphasia: Evidence for semantic processing , 1981, Brain and Language.

[25]  A. Albanese,et al.  Choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) activity differs in right and left human temporal lobes. , 1981, Neurology.

[26]  G. Pearlson,et al.  Suction lesions of the frontal cerebral cortex in the rat induce asymmetrical behavioral and catecholaminergic responses , 1981, Brain Research.

[27]  Y. Agid,et al.  Dopamine deficiency in the cerebral cortex in Parkinson disease , 1982, Neurology.

[28]  R. Katzman.,et al.  Intrusions as a sign of Alzheimer dementia chemical and pathological verification , 1982, Annals of neurology.

[29]  Sheila E. Blumstein,et al.  Semantic processing in aphasia: Evidence from an auditory lexical decision task , 1982, Brain and Language.

[30]  A. Lees,et al.  Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson's disease. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of visual attention , 1984 .

[32]  P. A. Williamson,et al.  Asymmetric neural control systems in human self-regulation. , 1984, Psychological review.

[33]  M. Albert,et al.  Varieties of perseveration , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Chemical lateralization in human temporal cortex. , 1984, Monographs in neural sciences.

[35]  K. Flowers,et al.  The effect of Parkinson's disease on the ability to maintain a mental set. , 1985, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.