Information Processing Biases in the Brain: Implications for Decision-Making and Self-Governance

To make behavioral choices that are in line with our goals and our moral beliefs, we need to gather and consider information about our current situation. Most information present in our environment is not relevant to the choices we need or would want to make and thus could interfere with our ability to behave in ways that reflect our underlying values. Certain sources of information could even lead us to make choices we later regret, and thus it would be beneficial to be able to ignore that information. Our ability to exert successful self-governance depends on our ability to attend to sources of information that we deem important to our decision-making processes. We generally assume that, at any moment, we have the ability to choose what we pay attention to. However, recent research indicates that what we pay attention to is influenced by our prior experiences, including reward history and past successes and failures, even when we are not aware of this history. Even momentary distractions can cause us to miss or discount information that should have a greater influence on our decisions given our values. Such biases in attention thus raise questions about the degree to which the choices that we make may be poorly informed and not truly reflect our ability to otherwise exert self-governance.

[1]  Anthony W. Sali,et al.  The role of reward prediction in the control of attention. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  J. Theeuwes Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[4]  K. Carpenter,et al.  Drug Stroop performance: relationships with primary substance of use and treatment outcome in a drug-dependent outpatient sample. , 2006, Addictive behaviors.

[5]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Embedding Reward Signals into Perception and Cognition , 2010, Front. Neurosci..

[6]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[7]  Steven Yantis,et al.  Attentional Bias for Non-drug Reward is Magnified in Addiction , 2014 .

[8]  John T Serences,et al.  Value-Based Modulations in Human Visual Cortex , 2008, Neuron.

[9]  J. Pearce,et al.  A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. , 1980, Psychological review.

[10]  Colin Camerer,et al.  The Attentional Drift-Diffusion Model Extends to Simple Purchasing Decisions , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[11]  A. Rangel,et al.  Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Hannah S. Locke,et al.  Prefrontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[13]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[14]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Visual Working Memory in Prefrontal Cortex of the Macaque , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  A. Mack Inattentional Blindness , 2003 .

[16]  Eva Zita Patai,et al.  Reward Associations Magnify Memory-based Biases on Perception , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  S. Yantis,et al.  Persistence of value-driven attentional capture. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Steven B. Most,et al.  Cognitive control and counterproductive oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli , 2015 .

[19]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Reward grabs the eye: Oculomotor capture by rewarding stimuli , 2012, Vision Research.

[22]  Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al.  Attention, Reward, and Information Seeking , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  TO SEE OR NOT TO SEE: The Need for Attention to Perceive Changes in Scenes , 1997 .

[24]  T. Braver,et al.  Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. , 2007 .

[25]  Jon Driver,et al.  Fortune and reversals of fortune in visual search: Reward contingencies for pop-out targets affect search efficiency and target repetition effects , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[26]  Tom Beesley,et al.  When goals conflict with values: counterproductive attentional and oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[27]  C. Koch,et al.  Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[28]  W. Schneider,et al.  Automatic attraction of attention to former targets in visual displays of letters , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  Rui M. Costa,et al.  Frontocerebellar Connectivity: Climbing through the Inferior Olive , 2010, Front. Neurosci..

[30]  K. Berridge,et al.  The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction , 1993, Brain Research Reviews.

[31]  John T Serences,et al.  Population response profiles in early visual cortex are biased in favor of more valuable stimuli. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  J. Rieskamp,et al.  Effective Connectivity between Hippocampus and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Controls Preferential Choices from Memory , 2015, Neuron.

[33]  W. van den Brink,et al.  Attentional bias predicts heroin relapse following treatment. , 2006, Addiction.

[34]  T. Egner,et al.  The Neural Underpinnings of How Reward Associations Can Both Guide and Misguide Attention , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  M. Desmurget,et al.  A parietal-premotor network for movement intention and motor awareness , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  Perceptual learning without perception , 2001, Nature.

[37]  C. N. Boehler,et al.  The influence of reward associations on conflict processing in the Stroop task , 2010, Cognition.

[38]  P. Goldman-Rakic Cellular basis of working memory , 1995, Neuron.

[39]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex. , 1998, Science.

[40]  Jatin G Vaidya,et al.  Value-Driven Attentional Capture in Adolescence , 2014, Psychological science.

[41]  P. Dayan,et al.  States versus Rewards: Dissociable Neural Prediction Error Signals Underlying Model-Based and Model-Free Reinforcement Learning , 2010, Neuron.

[42]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[43]  Rick A Adams,et al.  Age-related changes in working memory and the ability to ignore distraction , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  Steven Yantis,et al.  Value-driven attentional priority signals in human basal ganglia and visual cortex , 2014, Brain Research.

[45]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  It’s under control: Top-down search strategies can override attentional capture , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[46]  A. Arnsten Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[47]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Exogenous visual orienting by reward. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[48]  Joseph B. Sala,et al.  Binding of What and Where During Working Memory Maintenance , 2007, Cortex.

[49]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  Coordination of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Attentional Control in Human Cortex , 2005, Psychological science.

[50]  Pietro Perona,et al.  Optimal reward harvesting in complex perceptual environments , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[51]  Matthew L. Dixon,et al.  The Decision to Engage Cognitive Control Is Driven by Expected Reward-Value: Neural and Behavioral Evidence , 2012, PloS one.

[52]  Patryk A. Laurent,et al.  Generalization of value-based attentional priority , 2012, Visual cognition.

[53]  Dean F. Wong,et al.  The Role of Dopamine in Value-Based Attentional Orienting , 2016, Current Biology.

[54]  M. Field,et al.  Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. , 2008, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[55]  Steven Yantis,et al.  Value-driven attentional and oculomotor capture during goal-directed, unconstrained viewing , 2012, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[56]  Brian A Anderson,et al.  Variations in the magnitude of attentional capture: Testing a two-process model , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[57]  Emma H. Gail,et al.  Functional Genetic Variation in Dopamine Signaling Moderates Prefrontal Cortical Activity During Risky Decision Making , 2016, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[58]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Reward Guides Vision when It's Your Thing: Trait Reward-Seeking in Reward-Mediated Visual Priming , 2010, PloS one.

[59]  Steven Yantis,et al.  Learned Value Magnifies Salience-Based Attentional Capture , 2011, PloS one.

[60]  H. Egeth,et al.  Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[61]  B. Postle,et al.  Maintenance versus Manipulation of Information Held in Working Memory: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 1999, Brain and Cognition.

[62]  Jon Driver,et al.  Reward Priority of Visual Target Singletons Modulates Event-Related Potential Signatures of Attentional Selection , 2009, Psychological science.

[63]  Geoffrey F. Woodman,et al.  Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search , 1999, Nature.

[64]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Distractors that signal reward attract the eyes , 2015 .

[65]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex—An Update Time Is of the Essence , 2001, Neuron.

[66]  Patryk A. Laurent,et al.  Value-driven attentional capture , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[67]  Mengyuan Gong,et al.  Learned reward association improves visual working memory. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[68]  B. Campbell,et al.  Punishment and aversive behavior , 1969 .

[69]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  Perceptual learning rules based on reinforcers and attention , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[70]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[71]  Steven Yantis,et al.  Attentional bias for nondrug reward is magnified in addiction. , 2013, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[72]  J. Saiki,et al.  Task-irrelevant stimulus-reward association induces value-driven attentional capture , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[73]  S. Yantis,et al.  Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[74]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[75]  Ian Krajbich,et al.  Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[76]  Susan M. Courtney,et al.  Differential Neural Activation for Updating Rule versus Stimulus Information in Working Memory , 2008, Neuron.

[77]  M. Turatto,et al.  Reward associations impact both iconic and visual working memory , 2015, Vision Research.

[78]  B. Anderson A value-driven mechanism of attentional selection. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[79]  Sharif I. Kronemer,et al.  Reward, attention, and HIV-related risk in HIV+ individuals , 2016, Neurobiology of Disease.

[80]  Robert S Allison,et al.  The dichoptiscope: an instrument for investigating cues to motion in depth. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[81]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[82]  R. Desimone,et al.  Competitive Mechanisms Subserve Attention in Macaque Areas V2 and V4 , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[83]  J. Theeuwes Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[84]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Learning to Attend and to Ignore Is a Matter of Gains and Losses , 2009, Psychological science.

[85]  J. Raymond,et al.  Value Conditioning Modulates Visual Working Memory Processes , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[86]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[87]  Brian A. Anderson,et al.  Value-driven attentional priority is context specific , 2014, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[88]  Earl K. Miller,et al.  Selective representation of relevant information by neurons in the primate prefrontal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[89]  Graham V. Williams,et al.  Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[90]  K. C. Anderson,et al.  Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules , 2001, Nature.

[91]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory , 1997, Nature.

[92]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Behavioral/systems/cognitive Reward Changes Salience in Human Vision via the Anterior Cingulate , 2022 .

[93]  Fang Fang,et al.  Attention modulates neuronal correlates of interhemispheric integration and global motion perception. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[94]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[95]  Steven Yantis,et al.  Learned states of preparatory attentional control. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[96]  B. Anderson On the precision of goal-directed attentional selection. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[97]  Hyoung F. Kim,et al.  Why skill matters , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[98]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  Made you blink! Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.