Motor Adaptation Training for Faster Relearning

Adaptation is an error-driven motor learning process that can account for predictable changes in the environment (e.g., walking on ice) or in ourselves (e.g., injury). Our ability to recall and build upon adapted motor patterns across days is essential to this learning process. We investigated how different training paradigms affect the day-to-day memory of an adapted walking pattern. Healthy human adults walked on a split-belt treadmill, and returned the following day to assess recall, relearning rate, and performance. In the first experiment, one group adapted and de-adapted (i.e., washed-out the learning) several times on day 1 to practice the initial stage of learning where errors are large; another group adapted only one time and then practiced in the adapted (“learned”) state where errors were small. On day 2, they performed washout trials before readapting. The group that repeatedly practiced the initial portion of adaptation where errors are large showed the fastest relearning on the second day. In fact, the memory was nearly as strong as that of a third group that was left overnight in the adapted state and was not washed-out before reexposure on the second day. This demonstrates that alternating exposures to early adaptation and washout can enhance readaptation. In the second experiment, we tested whether the opposite split-belt pattern interferes with day 2 relearning. Surprisingly, it did not, and instead was similar to practicing in the adapted state. These results show that the structure of the initial phase of learning influences the ease of motor relearning.

[1]  M. Mauk,et al.  A Mechanism for Savings in the Cerebellum , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  S. M. Morton,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Locomotor Adaptations during Splitbelt Treadmill Walking , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Bartlett W. Mel,et al.  Cortical rewiring and information storage , 2004, Nature.

[4]  Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al.  Size of error affects cerebellar contributions to motor learning. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  A. Bastian,et al.  Thinking about walking: effects of conscious correction versus distraction on locomotor adaptation. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  E. Kandel,et al.  Cognitive Neuroscience and the Study of Memory , 1998, Neuron.

[7]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  Y. Dudai,et al.  Memory Extinction, Learning Anew, and Learning the New: Dissociations in the Molecular Machinery of Learning in Cortex , 2001, Science.

[9]  D. Reisman,et al.  Locomotor adaptation on a split-belt treadmill can improve walking symmetry post-stroke. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  R. Passingham,et al.  The Time Course of Changes during Motor Sequence Learning: A Whole-Brain fMRI Study , 1998, NeuroImage.

[11]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Adaptation to Visuomotor Transformations: Consolidation, Interference, and Forgetting , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Peter Mombaerts,et al.  From mind to molecule , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Anatomy of motor learning. II. Subcortical structures and learning by trial and error. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  J. Shea,et al.  Contextual interference effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill. , 1979 .

[15]  L Proteau,et al.  On the cognitive processes underlying contextual interference and observational learning. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  C. Winstein,et al.  Contextual Interference Effect: Elaborative Processing or Forgetting—Reconstruction? A Post Hoc Analysis of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation—Induced Effects on Motor Learning , 2008, Journal of motor behavior.

[17]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of Motor Performance and Motor Learning in the Primary Motor Cortex of Monkeys Adapting to an External Force Field , 2001, Neuron.

[18]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Split-belt treadmill adaptation shows different functional networks for fast and slow human walking. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Walking flexibility after hemispherectomy: split-belt treadmill adaptation and feedback control. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Consolidation of motor memory , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Visuomotor Skill Learning in Human Subjects Examined with Positron Emission Tomography , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[22]  Pablo Celnik,et al.  The University of Birmingham ( Live System ) Human locomotor adaptive learning is proportional to depression of cerebellar excitability , 2016 .

[23]  Vincent S. Huang,et al.  Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models , 2011, Neuron.

[24]  Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al.  Consolidation Patterns of Human Motor Memory , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Julia T. Choi,et al.  Adaptation reveals independent control networks for human walking , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  R. Miall,et al.  Adaptation to rotated visual feedback: a re-examination of motor interference , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  Rachael D Seidler,et al.  Neural Correlates of Motor Learning, Transfer of Learning, and Learning to Learn , 2010, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[28]  F. Samejima Cognitive processes. , 2021, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[29]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Failure to Consolidate the Consolidation Theory of Learning for Sensorimotor Adaptation Tasks , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  Hannah J. Block,et al.  Interlimb coordination during locomotion: what can be adapted and stored? , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  Motor Task Variation Induces Structural Learning , 2009, Current Biology.

[32]  Olivier White,et al.  Use-Dependent and Error-Based Learning of Motor Behaviors , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Current concepts in procedural consolidation , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  T. Brashers-Krug,et al.  Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  D. Reisman,et al.  Split-Belt Treadmill Adaptation Transfers to Overground Walking in Persons Poststroke , 2009, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[36]  John W Krakauer,et al.  Motor learning and consolidation: the case of visuomotor rotation. , 2009, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[37]  José María Delgado-García,et al.  Acquisition, Consolidation, Reconsolidation, and Extinction of Eyelid Conditioning Responses Require De Novo Protein Synthesis , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  P. Celnik,et al.  Dissociating the roles of the cerebellum and motor cortex during adaptive learning: the motor cortex retains what the cerebellum learns. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  E. Kandel The Molecular Biology of Memory Storage: A Dialogue Between Genes and Synapses , 2001, Science.

[40]  Dominic A Simon,et al.  Contextual Interference Effects with Two Tasks , 2007, Perceptual and motor skills.

[41]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[42]  E. Kandel,et al.  Memory: From Mind to Molecules , 1999 .

[43]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  Structure learning in action , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.