The social amplification of risk and the hazard sequence: the October 1995 oral contraceptive pill scare

Hazard notifications routinely occur as part of the identification or management of a hazard. It is argued that a series of such notifications – a hazard sequence – may affect public responses to future notifications about that hazard and also that hazard sequences can help explain patterns of risk amplification, particularly how a risk becomes normalised. Exploration of the hazard sequence also means exploring hazard templates: frameworks through which people make sense of risk information across the lifetime of the hazard. Events surrounding the 1995 oral contraceptive ‘pill scare’ are used to illustrate the way in which a hazard sequence might operate.

[1]  L. Rogers Pill scares and public responsibility [letter] , 1996 .

[2]  William J. Burns,et al.  The Social Amplification of Risk: Theoretical Foundations and Empirical Applications , 1992 .

[3]  K. Rothman,et al.  ORAL CONTRACEPTIVES AND BREAST CANCER , 1980 .

[4]  K. McPherson Arguments in editorial were not “biologically implausible” , 1999, BMJ.

[5]  I. Marková,et al.  Representations, Concepts and Social Change: The phenomenon of AIDS , 1987 .

[6]  M. Mitchell,et al.  THE IMPACT OF EXTERNAL PARTIES ON BRAND‐NAME CAPITAL: THE 1982 TYLENOL POISONINGS AND SUBSEQUENT CASES , 1989 .

[7]  R. Farmer,et al.  Effect of 1995 pill scare on rates of venous thromboembolism among women taking combined oral contraceptives: analysis of General Practice Research Database , 2000, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[8]  Julie Barnett,et al.  The Impact of Social Amplification on Risk Communication. Contract Research Report 322/2001 , 2001 .

[9]  K. Wellings Help or hype: an analysis of media coverage of the 1983 Pill scare. , 1985 .

[10]  Brian Wynne,et al.  Wising up : the public and new technologies , 2000 .

[11]  GPs were swamped by calls , 1995, BMJ.

[12]  W. Spitzer,et al.  Case-control study of oral contraceptives and risk of thromboembolic stroke: results from international study on oral contraceptives and health of young women , 1997, BMJ.

[13]  Valerie Beral,et al.  Mortality associated with oral contraceptive use: 25 year follow up of cohort of 46 000 women from Royal College of General Practitioners' oral contraception study , 1999, BMJ.

[14]  Glynis M. Breakwell,et al.  Mental models and social representations of hazards: the significance of identity processes , 2001 .

[15]  John C. Licciardone,et al.  Risk Communication and Public Health , 2000, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[16]  M. J. Quadrel,et al.  Risk perception and communication , 2008 .

[17]  Martin W. Bauer,et al.  Towards a Paradigm for Research on Social Representations , 1999 .

[18]  P. Slovic Trust, Emotion, Sex, Politics, and Science: Surveying the Risk‐Assessment Battlefield , 1999, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[19]  N. Pidgeon Risk communication and the social amplification of risk: theory, evidence and policy implications , 1999 .

[20]  J. Beniger,et al.  Pill and IUD discontinuation in the United States, 1970-1975: the influence of the media. , 1980, Family planning perspectives.

[21]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  World Health Statistics Quarterly , 1985 .

[22]  Ortwin Renn,et al.  The Social Amplification of Risk: A Conceptual Framework , 1988 .

[23]  R. Kasperson The social amplification of risk: progress in developing an integrative framework of risk’, in S. , 1992 .

[24]  A Furedi,et al.  The public health implications of the 1995 'pill scare'. , 1999, Human reproduction update.