The Interaction of Tactile Information and Movement Amplitude in a Multijoint Bimanual Circle-Tracing Task: Phase Transitions and Loss of Stability

Adaptive behaviour in bimanual coordination was examined with the use of a bimanual circle-tracing task. Circle diameter and tactile information were manipulated to form four tracing conditions: tracing a pair of 3-cm diameter circles with the tips of the index fingers (3F) or hand-held styli (3S) and tracing a pair of 10-cm diameter circles with the tips of the index fingers (10F) or hand-held styli (10S). Movement frequency was increased in all conditions. In the 3F, 3S, and 10S tracing conditions, an abrupt transition from asymmetric to symmetric coordination was the main adaptive response, while in the 10F tracing condition, phase wandering was the main adaptive response. Enhancement of fluctuations in relative phase, a signature of loss of stability, occurred before the transition from asymmetric to symmetric coordination. Movement frequency and movement amplitude interact as control parameters in this task. The results are discussed with reference to tactile surface contact and joint motion as sources of sensory information that can be used to stabilize bimanual coordination patterns. The presence or absence of tactile information is directly linked to the specific form of adaptive behaviour (phase transition or phase wandering) that emerges as a function of required movement amplitude and required pacing frequency.

[1]  M. T. Turvey,et al.  Models of interlimb coordination--equilibria, local analyses, and spectral patterning: comment on Fuchs and Kelso (1994). , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  R. Ivry,et al.  When two hands are better than one: reduced timing variability during bimanual movements. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  J. S. Scott Kelso,et al.  Haptic information stabilizes and destabilizes coordination dynamics , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[4]  J. Summers,et al.  Stabilisation of bimanual coordination through visual coupling , 1999 .

[5]  M. Akamatsu The influence of combined visual and tactile information on finger and eye movements during shape tracing. , 1992, Ergonomics.

[6]  Andras Semjen,et al.  Instabilities during antiphase bimanual movements: are ipsilateral pathways involved? , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  H N Zelaznik,et al.  The role of vision in repetitive circle drawing. , 1996, Acta psychologica.

[8]  J. Summers,et al.  The Dynamics of Bimanual Circle Drawing , 1997, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[9]  A. Vermeer,et al.  Bimanual circle drawing in children with spastic hemiparesis: effect of coupling modes on the performance of the impaired and unimpaired arms. , 2002, Acta psychologica.

[10]  M. Turvey,et al.  Symmetry, broken symmetry, and handedness in bimanual coordination dynamics , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  H. Haken,et al.  A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[12]  Knut Drewing,et al.  Reduced Timing Variability during Bimanual Coupling: A Role for Sensory Information , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[13]  Mingzhou Ding,et al.  Spontaneous recruitment and annihilation of degrees of freedom in biological coordination , 1993 .

[14]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Chapter 9 Going around in circles: The dynamics of bimanual circling , 1995 .

[15]  James W. Stinear,et al.  The subdominant hand increases the efficacy of voluntary alterations in bimanual coordination , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  A Semjen,et al.  Spontaneous and intentional pattern switching in a multisegmental bimanual coordination task. , 1999, Motor control.

[17]  J. Buchanan,et al.  Amplitude Scaling in a Bimanual Circle-Drawing Task: Pattern Switching and End-Effector Variability , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[18]  G Aschersleben,et al.  Tapping with peripheral nerve block. a role for tactile feedback in the timing of movements. , 2001, Experimental brain research.

[19]  Knut Drewing,et al.  The contribution of tactile reafference to temporal regularity during bimanual finger tapping , 2002, Psychological research.

[20]  J. A. Scott Kelso,et al.  Self-organization of trajectory formation , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.

[21]  M. Turvey,et al.  Handedness and the asymmetric dynamics of bimanual rhythmic coordination. , 1995 .

[22]  Gisa Aschersleben,et al.  A psychophysical approach to action timing , 2004 .

[23]  D E Sherwood,et al.  Hand preference, practice order, and spatial assimilations in rapid bimanual movement. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[24]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Attention as a mediating variable in the dynamics of bimanual coordination , 1996 .

[25]  J. Kelso Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.

[26]  Carol A. Putnam,et al.  On the Space-Time Structure of Human Interlimb Co-Ordination , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[27]  V K Jirsa,et al.  Recruitment of degrees of freedom stabilizes coordination. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  J. Kelso,et al.  Action-Perception as a Pattern Formation Process , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.

[29]  R. Ivry,et al.  Callosotomy patients exhibit temporal uncoupling during continuous bimanual movements , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  D. Glencross,et al.  Motor control and sensory motor integration : issues and directions , 1995 .

[31]  Mingzhou Ding,et al.  The spontaneous recruitment and suppression of degrees of freedom in rhythmic hand movements , 1997 .

[32]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Expressions of asymmetries and anchoring in bimanual coordination , 1994 .

[33]  S P Swinnen,et al.  Dissociating the structural and metrical specifications of bimanual movement. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.

[34]  R. Chua,et al.  Bimanual coordination dynamics in adults with Down syndrome. , 2002, Motor control.

[35]  J. Kelso,et al.  Multifrequency behavioral patterns and the phase attractive circle map , 1991, Biological Cybernetics.

[36]  J. Kelso,et al.  Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations , 1986 .

[37]  M. Turvey,et al.  Models of interlimb coordination--equilibria, local analyses, and spectral patterning: comment on Fuchs and Kelso (1994). , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  J. Kelso,et al.  A quantitative approach to understanding the formation and change of coordinated movement patterns. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[39]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Proprioceptive control of multijoint movement: bimanual circle drawing , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  J. Kelso,et al.  To Switch or Not to Switch: Recruitment of Degrees of Freedom Stabilizes Biological Coordination. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[41]  Daniel Cattaert,et al.  Hand coordination in bimanual circle drawing. , 1995 .