When endogenous spatial attention improves conscious perception: Effects of alerting and bottom-up activation

Recent studies have consistently demonstrated that conscious perception interacts with exogenous attentional orienting, but it can be dissociated from endogenous attentional orienting (Chica Lasaponara, et al., 2011; Wyart & Tallon-Baudry, 2008). It has been hypothesized that enhanced conscious processing at exogenously attended locations results from a synergistic action of spatial orienting, bottom-up activation, and phasic alerting induced by the abrupt onset of the exogenous cue (Chica, Lasaponara, et al., 2011). Instead, as endogenous cues need more time to be interpreted, the phasic alerting they produce may have dissipated when the target appears. Furthermore, endogenous cues presumably elicit a weak bottom-up activation at the cued location. Consistent with these hypotheses, we observed that endogenous attention modulated conscious perception, but only when phasic alerting or bottom-up activation was increased. Results are discussed in the context of recent theoretical models of consciousness (Dehaene, Changeux, Naccache, Sackur, & Sergent, 2006).

[1]  C. Koch,et al.  Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? , 1995, Nature.

[2]  J. Jonides Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .

[3]  R. Ulrich,et al.  Visual attention and temporal discrimination: Differential effects of automatic and voluntary cueing , 2006 .

[4]  Catherine Tallon-Baudry,et al.  On the Neural Mechanisms Subserving Consciousness and Attention , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[5]  J B Poline,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  J. Changeux,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.5 May 2006 Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy , 2022 .

[7]  J. Changeux,et al.  Experimental and Theoretical Approaches to Conscious Processing , 2011, Neuron.

[8]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  Phasic auditory alerting improves visual conscious perception , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[9]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  V. Lamme,et al.  The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[11]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Attention networks and their interactions after right-hemisphere damage , 2012, Cortex.

[12]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[13]  P. C. Murphy,et al.  Cerebral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral Cortex.

[14]  E. Macaluso,et al.  Multisensory spatial interactions: a window onto functional integration in the human brain , 2005, Trends in Neurosciences.

[15]  R. W. Kentridge,et al.  Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  M. Carrasco,et al.  On the flexibility of sustained attention and its effects on a texture segmentation task , 2008, Vision Research.

[17]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .

[18]  J. Buhle,et al.  Typologies of attentional networks , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  B. Milliken,et al.  Separate mechanisms recruited by exogenous and endogenous spatial cues: evidence from a spatial Stroop paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  G. Calvert,et al.  Multisensory integration: methodological approaches and emerging principles in the human brain , 2004, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[21]  Lamme Vaf,et al.  Why visual attention and awareness are different , 2003 .

[22]  Neelam Naikar,et al.  A new component of visual orienting: Implicit effects of peripheral information and subthreshold cues on covert attention , 1999 .

[23]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Spatial attention and conscious perception: Interactions and dissociations between and within endogenous and exogenous processes , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  Ana B. Chica,et al.  Attentional Routes to Conscious Perception , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[25]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Effects of temporal cueing on residual visual discrimination in blindsight , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  C. Tallon-Baudry,et al.  Neural Dissociation between Visual Awareness and Spatial Attention , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Neural correlates of consciousness in humans , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[28]  Emiliano Macaluso,et al.  Spatial attention can modulate audiovisual integration at multiple cortical and subcortical sites , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  N. Bolognini,et al.  “Acoustical vision” of below threshold stimuli: interaction among spatially converging audiovisual inputs , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[31]  Michael T. Lippert,et al.  Improvement of visual contrast detection by a simultaneous sound , 2007, Brain Research.

[32]  R. W. Kentridge,et al.  Attended but unseen: Visual attention is not sufficient for visual awareness , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness , 1998, Nature.

[34]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Spatial attention and conscious perception: the role of endogenous and exogenous orienting , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[35]  Pío Tudela,et al.  Modulations among the alerting, orienting and executive control networks , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[37]  C. Koch,et al.  Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews the Spatial Orienting Paradigm: How to Design and Interpret Spatial Attention Experiments , 2022 .

[39]  Juan Lupiáñez,et al.  Two cognitive and neural systems for endogenous and exogenous spatial attention , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[40]  W. Schneider,et al.  How does phasic alerting improve performance in patients with unilateral neglect? A systematic analysis of attentional processing capacity and spatial weighting mechanisms , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  P. McCormick Orienting attention without awareness. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  Viola S. Störmer,et al.  Salient Sounds Activate Human Visual Cortex Automatically , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  Paolo Bartolomeo,et al.  Dorsal and Ventral Parietal Contributions to Spatial Orienting in the Human Brain , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Attention and the multiple stages of multisensory integration: A review of audiovisual studies. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[45]  I. THE ATTENTION SYSTEM OF THE HUMAN BRAIN , 2002 .

[46]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Testing the Efficiency and Independence of Attentional Networks , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  Robert W. Kentridge,et al.  Attention without awareness in blindsight , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[48]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[49]  Paolo Bartolomeo,et al.  Causal contributions of the left frontal eye field to conscious perception. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Unconscious Masked Priming Depends on Temporal Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[51]  Paolo Bartolomeo,et al.  Neural bases of the interactions between spatial attention and conscious perception. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.