Visual objects interact differently during encoding and memory maintenance
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Srimant P Tripathy,et al. High-capacity, transient retention of direction-of-motion information for multiple moving objects. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[2] R. Sekuler,et al. Similarity-based distortion of visual short-term memory is due to perceptual averaging , 2014, Vision Research.
[3] C. Paffen,et al. Visual Working Memory Storage Recruits Sensory Processing Areas , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[4] Elkan G. Akyürek,et al. Dynamic hidden states underlying working memory guided behaviour , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.
[5] Juha Silvanto,et al. How Visual Short-Term Memory Maintenance Modulates Subsequent Visual Aftereffects , 2013, Psychological science.
[6] A. Compte,et al. Build-up of serial dependence in color working memory , 2018, Scientific Reports.
[7] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Neural Evidence for a Distinction between Short-term Memory and the Focus of Attention , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[8] A. Hollingworth. Scene and position specificity in visual memory for objects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[9] Peter Wenderoth,et al. The different mechanisms of the motion direction illusion and aftereffect , 2007, Vision Research.
[10] Zoltán Vidnyánszky,et al. Retention interval affects visual short-term memory encoding. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[11] A. Kohn,et al. Distinct Effects of Brief and Prolonged Adaptation on Orientation Tuning in Primary Visual Cortex , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[12] Jason M. Scimeca,et al. Reaffirming the Sensory Recruitment Account of Working Memory , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[13] Visual Memories Bypass Normalization , 2018, Psychological science.
[14] Ehud Zohary,et al. Turning Symbolic: The Representation of Motion Direction in Working Memory , 2016, Front. Psychol..
[15] Viola S. Störmer,et al. Feature-Based Attention Elicits Surround Suppression in Feature Space , 2014, Current Biology.
[16] C. Blakemore,et al. Lateral inhibition between orientation detectors in the cat's visual cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] Duje Tadin,et al. Perceptual and neural consequences of rapid motion adaptation , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[18] Liqiang Huang,et al. Characterizing the Nature of Visual Conscious Access: The Distinction between Features and Locations , 2010, Journal of vision.
[19] C Y Li,et al. Shift in speed selectivity of visual cortical neurons: a neural basis of perceived motion contrast. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[20] Mark D'Esposito,et al. Flexible Coding of Visual Working Memory Representations during Distraction , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[21] N. Qian,et al. Axis-of-motion affects direction discrimination, not speed discrimination , 1999, Vision Research.
[22] A. Hollingworth,et al. Dynamic interactions between visual working memory and saccade target selection. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[23] Joonkoo Park,et al. Serial dependence in numerosity perception , 2018, Journal of vision.
[24] Jochen Triesch,et al. What’s “up”? Working memory contents can bias orientation processing , 2013, Vision Research.
[25] S. Luck,et al. Sudden Death and Gradual Decay in Visual Working Memory , 2009, Psychological science.
[26] William Curran,et al. The hierarchy of directional interactions in visual motion processing , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[27] Yaoda Xu. Sensory Cortex Is Nonessential in Working Memory Storage , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[28] Vanda Agnes Nemes,et al. A psychophysical investigation of human visual perceptual memory : a study of the retention of colour, spatial frequency and motion visual information by human visual short term memory mechanisms , 2011 .
[29] Peter Wenderoth,et al. Dichoptic reduction of the direction illusion is not due to binocular rivalry , 2010, Vision Research.
[30] David Whitaker,et al. The retention and disruption of color information in human short-term visual memory. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[31] V. Lollo,et al. Temporal characteristics of iconic memory , 1977, Nature.
[32] R. Sekuler,et al. Adaptation alters perceived direction of motion , 1976, Vision Research.
[33] Paul M Bays,et al. The precision of visual working memory is set by allocation of a shared resource. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[34] John T. Serences,et al. Coexisting representations of sensory and mnemonic information in human visual cortex , 2019, Nature Neuroscience.
[35] Robert Patterson,et al. Direction-selective adaptation and simultaneous contrast induced by stereoscopic (cyclopean) motion , 1996, Vision Research.
[36] Haluk Öğmen,et al. Attention and non-retinotopic feature integration. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[37] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Perceptual manifestations of fast neural plasticity: Motion priming, rapid motion aftereffect and perceptual sensitization , 2005, Vision Research.
[38] M. Coltheart,et al. Iconic memory and visible persistence , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.
[39] Nicholas E. Myers,et al. Supraliminal But Not Subliminal Distracters Bias Working Memory Recall , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[40] Stefan Czoschke,et al. Context information supports serial dependence of multiple visual objects across memory episodes , 2019, bioRxiv.
[41] D Zaksas,et al. Motion information is spatially localized in a visual working-memory task. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[42] Yaoda Xu. Reevaluating the Sensory Account of Visual Working Memory Storage , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[43] Edward Awh,et al. Alpha-Band Activity Reveals Spontaneous Representations of Spatial Position in Visual Working Memory , 2017, Current Biology.
[44] R. Sekuler,et al. Mutual repulsion between moving visual targets. , 1979, Science.
[45] Michele Fornaciai,et al. Spontaneous repulsive adaptation in the absence of attractive serial dependence. , 2019, Journal of vision.
[46] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[47] Adam C. Riggall,et al. Within-Category Decoding of Information in Different Attentional States in Short-Term Memory , 2016, Cerebral cortex.
[48] William Curran,et al. Direction Repulsion Goes Global , 2003, Current Biology.
[49] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Visual persistence of figures defined by relative motion , 1992, Vision Research.
[50] P. Wenderoth,et al. Retinotopic encoding of the direction aftereffect , 2008, Vision Research.
[51] Daniel P. Bliss,et al. Serial dependence is absent at the time of perception but increases in visual working memory , 2017, bioRxiv.
[52] P. Bays,et al. Visual Working Memory Is Independent of the Cortical Spacing Between Memoranda , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[53] B Moulden,et al. A Simultaneous Shift in Apparent Direction: Further Evidence for a “Distribution-Shift” Model of Direction Coding , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[54] G. Mather. The Movement Aftereffect and a Distribution-Shift Model for Coding the Direction of Visual Movement , 1980, Perception.
[55] Randolph Blake,et al. Visual working memory contaminates perception , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[56] Edgar Erdfelder,et al. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[57] R. Wurtz,et al. Response to motion in extrastriate area MSTl: center-surround interactions. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[58] H. Wilson,et al. Motion Integration over Space: Interaction of the Center and Surround Motion* * This research was first reported at the Annual Meeting of the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, May 1994 and 1995. , 1997, Vision Research.
[59] O. Braddick,et al. The temporal integration and resolution of velocity signals , 1991, Vision Research.
[60] C. Gilbert. Horizontal integration and cortical dynamics , 1992, Neuron.
[61] Yoshimichi Ejima,et al. Apparent contrast of a sinusoidal grating in the simultaneous presence of peripheral gratings , 1985, Vision Research.
[62] Dag Alnæs,et al. Modulation of Activity in Human Visual Area V1 during Memory Masking , 2011, PloS one.
[63] F. Tong,et al. Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas , 2009, Nature.
[64] Roger B. H. Tootell,et al. Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area , 1992, Nature.
[65] E. Boring,et al. The derivation of subjective scales from just noticeable differences. , 1958, Psychological review.
[66] Edward F. Ester,et al. Restoring Latent Visual Working Memory Representations in Human Cortex , 2016, Neuron.
[67] V. Lollo. Temporal integration in visual memory. , 1980 .
[68] C. Blakemore,et al. Lateral Inhibition between Orientation Detectors in the Human Visual System , 1970, Nature.
[69] Leonardo Chelazzi,et al. Laws of concatenated perception: Vision goes for novelty, decisions for perseverance , 2017, bioRxiv.
[70] S. Petersen,et al. Memory's echo: vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[71] John-Dylan Haynes,et al. Decoding the Contents of Visual Short-Term Memory from Human Visual and Parietal Cortex , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[72] William Curran,et al. The direction aftereffect is driven by adaptation of local motion detectors , 2006, Vision Research.
[73] Rasa Ruseckaite,et al. Human scotopic dark adaptation: Comparison of recoveries of psychophysical threshold and ERG b-wave sensitivity. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[74] Rosanne L Rademaker,et al. The impact of interference on short-term memory for visual orientation. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[75] S. Luck,et al. Discrete fixed-resolution representations in visual working memory , 2008, Nature.
[76] Edward F. Ester,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Stimulus-Specific Delay Activity in Human Primary Visual Cortex , 2022 .
[77] Vasudevan Lakshminarayanan,et al. Psychophysical estimation of speed discrimination. I. Methodology. , 2005, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[78] Adam C. Riggall,et al. The Relationship between Working Memory Storage and Elevated Activity as Measured with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[79] T. Sejnowski,et al. Representation of Color Stimuli in Awake Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 2003, Neuron.
[80] Rosilari Bellacosa Marotti,et al. The temporal course of recovery from brief (sub-second) adaptations to spatial contrast , 2012, Vision Research.
[81] Gerald Westhhmer,et al. Simultaneous orientation contrast for lines in the human fovea , 1990, Vision Research.
[82] S. Appelle. Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the "oblique effect" in man and animals. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.
[83] P. Dayan,et al. Space and time in visual context , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[84] B. L. Gros,et al. Anisotropies in visual motion perception: a fresh look. , 1998, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[85] Xin Huang,et al. Distributed and Dynamic Neural Encoding of Multiple Motion Directions of Transparently Moving Stimuli in Cortical Area MT , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[86] Peter Wenderoth,et al. The Hierarchical Order of Processes Underlying the Direction Illusion and the Direction Aftereffect , 2012, Perception.
[87] Thomas Wachtler,et al. "Tilt" in color space: Hue changes induced by chromatic surrounds. , 2015, Journal of vision.
[88] F. D. Lange,et al. Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision , 2017, Current Biology.
[89] Frank Tong,et al. Spatial specificity of working memory representations in the early visual cortex. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[90] Nicholas E. Myers,et al. Benefits of Flexible Prioritization in Working Memory Can Arise Without Costs , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[91] K. Nakayama,et al. A Velocity Analogue of Brightness Contrast , 1973, Perception.
[92] A. Hollingworth. Object-position binding in visual memory for natural scenes and object arrays. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[93] Min-Suk Kang,et al. Retrieval-Induced Inhibition in Short-Term Memory , 2015, Psychological science.
[94] Adam C. Riggall,et al. Distributed Patterns of Activity in Sensory Cortex Reflect the Precision of Multiple Items Maintained in Visual Short-Term Memory , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[95] R. Sekuler,et al. Distortions in recall from visual memory: two classes of attractors at work. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[96] Katsumi Watanabe,et al. Shape-assimilation effect: retrospective distortion of visual shapes , 2013, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[97] T. Womelsdorf,et al. Feature-based attention influences contextual interactions during motion repulsion , 2006, Vision Research.
[98] Paul M Bays,et al. Evaluating and excluding swap errors in analogue tests of working memory , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[99] Timothy F. Brady,et al. Modeling visual working memory with the MemToolbox. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[100] D. Whitney,et al. Serial Dependence in the Perception of Faces , 2014, Current Biology.
[101] Masud Husain,et al. Rapid Forgetting Results From Competition Over Time Between Items in Visual Working Memory , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[102] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[103] S. Luck,et al. Interactions between visual working memory representations , 2017, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[104] William Curran,et al. Directional performance in motion transparency , 2002, Vision Research.
[105] G. Orban,et al. Shape and Spatial Distribution of Receptive Fields and Antagonistic Motion Surrounds in the Middle Temporal Area (V5) of the Macaque , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.
[106] P. Wenderoth,et al. The tilt illusion as a function of the relative and absolute lengths of test and inducing lines , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.
[107] Bahador Bahrami,et al. Precision of working memory for visual motion sequences and transparent motion surfaces. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[108] Wataru Teramoto,et al. Visual percepts modify iconic memory in humans , 2018, Scientific Reports.
[109] David Alais,et al. Temporal Integration of Movement: The Time-Course of Motion Streaks Revealed by Masking , 2011, PloS one.
[110] D. Whitney,et al. Serial dependence in visual perception , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[111] J. Allman,et al. Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.
[112] J. Kaiser,et al. Two types of serial dependence in visual working memory. , 2018, British journal of psychology.
[113] D. Whitaker,et al. Multiple spatial frequency channels in human visual perceptual memory , 2011, Vision Research.