A Model for Integrating Elementary Neural Functions into Delayed-Response Behavior

It is well established that various cortical regions can implement a wide array of neural processes, yet the mechanisms which integrate these processes into behavior-producing, brain-scale activity remain elusive. We propose that an important role in this respect might be played by executive structures controlling the traffic of information between the cortical regions involved. To illustrate this hypothesis, we present a neural network model comprising a set of interconnected structures harboring stimulus-related activity (visual representation, working memory, and planning), and a group of executive units with task-related activity patterns that manage the information flowing between them. The resulting dynamics allows the network to perform the dual task of either retaining an image during a delay (delayed-matching to sample task), or recalling from this image another one that has been associated with it during training (delayed-pair association task). The model reproduces behavioral and electrophysiological data gathered on the inferior temporal and prefrontal cortices of primates performing these same tasks. It also makes predictions on how neural activity coding for the recall of the image associated with the sample emerges and becomes prospective during the training phase. The network dynamics proves to be very stable against perturbations, and it exhibits signs of scale-invariant organization and cooperativity. The present network represents a possible neural implementation for active, top-down, prospective memory retrieval in primates. The model suggests that brain activity leading to performance of cognitive tasks might be organized in modular fashion, simple neural functions becoming integrated into more complex behavior by executive structures harbored in prefrontal cortex and/or basal ganglia.

[1]  E. Miller,et al.  Task-specific neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  T. Gisiger Scale invariance in biology: coincidence or footprint of a universal mechanism? , 2001, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[3]  J. Desmond,et al.  Prefrontal regions involved in keeping information in and out of mind. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Top-down signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of memory retrieval , 1999, Nature.

[5]  Yasushi Miyashita,et al.  Cognitive Memory: Cellular and Network Machineries and Their Top-Down Control , 2004, Science.

[6]  E. Miller,et al.  Neural circuits subserving the retrieval and maintenance of abstract rules. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Visual Working Memory in Prefrontal Cortex of the Macaque , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  E. Miller,et al.  Neural Activity in the Primate Prefrontal Cortex during Associative Learning , 1998, Neuron.

[9]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Planning and Spatial Working Memory: a Positron Emission Tomography Study in Humans , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[10]  P. Strick,et al.  The temporal lobe is a target of output from the basal ganglia. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  R. Passingham,et al.  Active maintenance in prefrontal area 46 creates distractor-resistant memory , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  Niraj S. Desai,et al.  Activity-dependent scaling of quantal amplitude in neocortical neurons , 1998, Nature.

[13]  Masahiko Morita,et al.  Computational modeling of pair-association memory in inferior temporal cortex. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[14]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Visuospatial coding in primate prefrontal neurons revealed by oculomotor paradigms. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  B. Horwitz,et al.  Integrating electrophysiological and anatomical experimental data to create a large-scale model that simulates a delayed match-to-sample human brain imaging study. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  F. Attneave,et al.  The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory , 1949 .

[17]  B. Berger,et al.  Catecholamine innervation of the human cerebral cortex as revealed by comparative immunohistochemistry of tyrosine hydroxylase and dopamine‐beta‐hydroxylase , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[18]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .

[19]  R. Passingham,et al.  The prefrontal cortex: response selection or maintenance within working memory? , 2000, 5th IEEE EMBS International Summer School on Biomedical Imaging, 2002..

[20]  J. Joyce,et al.  Dopamine D2 receptors are organized in bands in normal human temporal cortex , 1996, Neuroscience.

[21]  John Duncan,et al.  A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex , 1993, Nature.

[22]  Michel Kerszberg,et al.  Acquisition and performance of delayed-response tasks: a neural network model. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  P. Strick,et al.  Basal-ganglia 'projections' to the prefrontal cortex of the primate. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Neural organization for the long-term memory of paired associates , 1991, Nature.

[25]  Jonas Rose,et al.  Neural Correlates of Executive Control in the Avian Brain , 2005, PLoS biology.

[26]  E. Bienenstock,et al.  Theory for the development of neuron selectivity: orientation specificity and binocular interaction in visual cortex , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[27]  Nicolas Brunel,et al.  Dynamics and plasticity of stimulus-selective persistent activity in cortical network models. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  A M Graybiel,et al.  The basal ganglia and adaptive motor control. , 1994, Science.

[29]  W. Schultz,et al.  Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory , 2001, Nature.

[30]  A. Dagher,et al.  Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  E. Miller,et al.  Prospective Coding for Objects in Primate Prefrontal Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  K. C. Anderson,et al.  Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules , 2001, Nature.

[33]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Neuronal correlate of pictorial short-term memory in the primate temporal cortexYasushi Miyashita , 1988, Nature.

[34]  M. Kerszberg Genetics and Epigenetics of Neural Function: A Model , 1990, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  Yoshikazu Isomura,et al.  Neural Coding of “Attention for Action” and “Response Selection” in Primate Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  A. Graybiel Building action repertoires: memory and learning functions of the basal ganglia , 1995, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[37]  Boris S. Gutkin,et al.  Turning On and Off with Excitation: The Role of Spike-Timing Asynchrony and Synchrony in Sustained Neural Activity , 2001, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[38]  B. Berger,et al.  Regional and laminar distribution of the dopamine and serotonin innervation in the macaque cerebral cortex: A radioautographic study , 1988, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[39]  D. Amit,et al.  Retrospective and prospective persistent activity induced by Hebbian learning in a recurrent cortical network , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[40]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Backward spreading of memory-retrieval signal in the primate temporal cortex. , 2001, Science.

[41]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Activity of primate inferotemporal neurons related to a sought target in pair-association task. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[42]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Neuron Activity Related to Short-Term Memory , 1971, Science.