Perception and Eye Movements

Over the last decade or so, revolution in our understanding of the neural processing underlying oculomotor behavior has begun. Here we review recent evidence that nonretinal signals related to perception and cognition serve as core components of the visual drive for voluntary eye movements. Older models that merely manipulate raw retinal signals within quasilinear negative feedback loops in retinal coordinates cannot expain a host of revealing new findings. New models of pursuit and saccades that incorporate nonlinear processing of signals more closely related the properties of target objects, within coordinate system more closely related to head or world coordinates, are needed to explain how top-down cognitive factors and bottom-up retinal data combine to drive both perception and oculomotor action.

[1]  Leland S. Stone,et al.  Expansion of Direction Space around the Cardinal Axes Revealed by Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 2005, Neuron.

[2]  Uwe J Ilg,et al.  Commentary: smooth pursuit eye movements: from low-level to high-level vision. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[3]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  The influence of the Brentano illusion on eye and hand movements. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[4]  Christopher D. Carello,et al.  Target selection and the superior colliculus: goals, choices and hypotheses , 2004, Vision Research.

[5]  Peter H Schiller,et al.  Neural mechanisms underlying target selection with saccadic eye movements. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[6]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  Saccadic and perceptual performance in visual search tasks. I. Contrast detection and discrimination. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[7]  E. Brenner,et al.  10 years of illusions. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  Similar Neural Representations of the Target for Saccades and Perception during Search , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  The effects of optical compression and magnification on distance estimation , 2010 .

[10]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Shared motion signals for human perceptual decisions and oculomotor actions. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[11]  P. Thier,et al.  Posterior Parietal Cortex Neurons Encode Target Motion in World-Centered Coordinates , 2004, Neuron.

[12]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Saccades, salience and attention: the role of the lateral intraparietal area in visual behavior. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[13]  Eric W. Sellers,et al.  How are elements of a scenic layout bound together , 2010 .

[14]  Paul Dassonville,et al.  Perception, Action, and Roelofs Effect: A Mere Illusion of Dissociation , 2004, PLoS biology.

[15]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[16]  D. Sparks The brainstem control of saccadic eye movements , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  Richard J Krauzlis,et al.  Pursuit of the ineffable: perceptual and motor reversals during the tracking of apparent motion. , 2003, Journal of Vision.

[19]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[20]  J Lorenceau,et al.  Visual Motion Integration for Perception and Pursuit , 2000, Perception.

[21]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Tracking with the mind’s eye , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[22]  R. Krauzlis The Control of Voluntary Eye Movements: New Perspectives , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.