Acute visual neglect and extinction: distinct functional state of the visuospatial attention system.

The neural mechanisms underlying spatial neglect are still disputed. Abnormal left parietal hyperactivation is proposed to lead to the rightward attentional bias, a clinical hallmark of neglect. Extinction, another deficit of visuospatial attention, is regarded as either a 'mild' form of neglect or a distinct syndrome. Although both neglect and extinction are typical syndromes of acute right hemispheric stroke, all imaging studies investigating these syndromes were conducted at least several weeks after stroke onset, in a phase when brain reorganization has already progressed. The present study aimed at comparing the activation patterns in acute stroke patients with neglect and extinction during visuospatial processing. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we examined the functional state of the attention system in 33 patients with a first ever stroke (53 ± 5 h after stroke onset) and age-matched healthy subjects (n = 15). All patients had embolic infarcts within the territory of the right middle cerebral artery. Patients were divided into three groups: (i) normal visuospatial processing (control patients, n = 11); (ii) patients with visual extinction but with no signs of neglect (n = 9); and (iii) patients with visual neglect (n = 13). While undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging, patients performed a Posner-like task for visuospatial attention with detection of the targets in the left and right visual hemifields. Patients with neglect showed the expected imbalance in the left versus right parietal activation, which however, was present also in control and extinction patients, thus representing an epiphenomenon of the acute structural lesion in the right hemisphere. Compared with control patients, neglect was characterized by reduced activation in the right parietal and lateral occipital cortex, as well as in the left frontal eye field. In contrast, the activation pattern in patients with extinction differed from all other groups by an increased activation of the left prefrontal cortex. In both patients with neglect and extinction, detection of targets in the left hemifield correlated with an activation in the left prefrontal and parietal cortex. Thus at least in acute stroke, a relative hyperactivation of the left parietal cortex is not a particular characteristic of neglect. The specific signature of neglect is represented by the dysfunction of the right parietal and lateral occipital cortex. The function of the left attentional centres might provide a compensatory role after critical right hemisphere lesions and be relevant for the contralesional spatial processing.

[1]  P. Weiss,et al.  Visual extinction in relation to visuospatial neglect after right-hemispheric stroke: quantitative assessment and statistical lesion-symptom mapping , 2011, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[2]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  The Role of Contralesional Dorsal Premotor Cortex after Stroke as Studied with Concurrent TMS-fMRI , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Thomas Nägele,et al.  The Role of Temporo-parietal Cortex in Subcortical Visual Extinction , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  K. Lovblad,et al.  Neuroanatomy of hemispatial neglect and its functional components: a study using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  C. Weiller,et al.  Structural connectivity for visuospatial attention: significance of ventral pathways. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  Y. Tabei,et al.  Damage to the right superior longitudinal fasciculus in the inferior parietal lobe plays a role in spatial neglect , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  D. Levine,et al.  UNILATERAL NEGLECT IS MORE SEVERE AND COMMON IN OLDER PATIENTS WITH RIGHT HEMISPHERIC STROKE , 2009, Neurology.

[8]  H. Ackermann,et al.  The role of the unaffected hemisphere in motor recovery after stroke , 2009, NeuroImage.

[9]  Jan Gläscher,et al.  Visualization of Group Inference Data in Functional Neuroimaging , 2009, Neuroinformatics.

[10]  J. Rothwell,et al.  Hyperexcitability of parietal-motor functional connections in the intact left-hemisphere of patients with neglect. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  J. Driver,et al.  Abnormal Attentional Modulation of Retinotopic Cortex in Parietal Patients with Spatial Neglect , 2008, Current Biology.

[12]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  White matter (dis)connections and gray matter (dys)functions in visual neglect: Gaining insights into the brain networks of spatial awareness , 2008, Cortex.

[13]  Suzanne T. Witt,et al.  Functional neuroimaging correlates of finger-tapping task variations: An ALE meta-analysis , 2008, NeuroImage.

[14]  Katherine L. Perdue,et al.  Enhanced cortical activation in the contralesional hemisphere of chronic stroke patients in response to motor skill challenge. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  G. Fink,et al.  Neural correlates of recovery from acute hemispatial neglect. , 2008, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[16]  Darren Gitelman,et al.  Anatomical physiology of spatial extinction. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  P Bartolomeo,et al.  Brain networks of spatial awareness: evidence from diffusion tensor imaging tractography , 2007, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry.

[18]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  Left unilateral neglect as a disconnection syndrome. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial normalization of lesioned brains: Performance evaluation and impact on fMRI analyses , 2007, NeuroImage.

[20]  Umberto Sabatini,et al.  Neural bases of personal and extrapersonal neglect in humans. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric mapping , 2013 .

[22]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  CHAPTER 2 – Statistical parametric mapping , 2007 .

[23]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Visual Attention : How Top-Down Feedback Highlights Relevant Locations , 2007 .

[24]  A. Ellis,et al.  Unilateral Neglect is not Unilateral: Evidence for Additional Neglect of Extreme Right Space , 2006, Cortex.

[25]  Argye E. Hillis,et al.  Neural Correlates of Modality-specific Spatial Extinction , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  Oliver Speck,et al.  Magnetic resonance imaging of freely moving objects: prospective real-time motion correction using an external optical motion tracking system , 2006, NeuroImage.

[27]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Functional anatomy of the therapeutic effects of prism adaptation on left neglect , 2006, Neurology.

[28]  Elizabeth Coulthard,et al.  Visual neglect after right posterior cerebral artery infarction , 2006, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[29]  C. Weiller,et al.  Dynamics of language reorganization after stroke. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  H. Karnath,et al.  Disturbed line bisection is associated with posterior brain lesions , 2006, Brain Research.

[31]  M. Hallett,et al.  Multimodal imaging of brain reorganization in motor areas of the contralesional hemisphere of well recovered patients after capsular stroke. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  M. Jehkonen,et al.  Impact of neglect on functional outcome after stroke: a review of methodological issues and recent research findings. , 2006, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[33]  G A Orban,et al.  Attentional responses to unattended stimuli in human parietal cortex. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Unified segmentation , 2005, NeuroImage.

[35]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data , 2005, NeuroImage.

[36]  Peter B Barker,et al.  Anatomy of Spatial Attention: Insights from Perfusion Imaging and Hemispatial Neglect in Acute Stroke , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Jon Driver,et al.  Spatial working memory capacity in unilateral neglect. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Neural basis and recovery of spatial attention deficits in spatial neglect , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  Klaas E. Stephan,et al.  Visuospatial attention: how to measure effects of infrequent, unattended events in a blocked stimulus design , 2004, NeuroImage.

[40]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  Right spatial neglect after left hemisphere stroke , 2004, Neurology.

[41]  M Zaitsev,et al.  Point spread function mapping with parallel imaging techniques and high acceleration factors: Fast, robust, and flexible method for echo‐planar imaging distortion correction , 2004, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[42]  Chris Rorden,et al.  The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 patients. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  W R Clarke,et al.  Frequency, risk factors, anatomy, and course of unilateral neglect in an acute stroke cohort , 2004, Neurology.

[44]  K. Krug A common neuronal code for perceptual processes in visual cortex? Comparing choice and attentional correlates in V5/MT. , 2004, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[45]  Thomas T. Liu,et al.  Part II: design of experiments , 2022 .

[46]  Thomas T. Liu,et al.  Efficiency, power, and entropy in event-related FMRI with multiple trial types Part I: theory , 2004, NeuroImage.

[47]  Christopher Kennard,et al.  Visual neglect associated with frontal lobe infarction , 1996, Journal of Neurology.

[48]  C. Kennard,et al.  The anatomy of visual neglect. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[49]  Cornelius Weiller,et al.  The influence of extra- and intracranial artery disease on the BOLD signal in FMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[50]  Wilhelm Küker,et al.  The cortical substrate of visual extinction , 2003, Neuroreport.

[51]  J. Marshall,et al.  Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  R. Ridley,et al.  Contralesional neglect in monkeys with small unilateral parietal cortical ablations , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[53]  P. Matthews,et al.  The role of ipsilateral premotor cortex in hand movement after stroke , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[54]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Neural Correlates of Conscious and Unconscious Vision in Parietal Extinction , 2002, Neurocase.

[55]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[56]  J. Drivera,et al.  Neural response to emotional faces with and without awareness : event-related fMRI in a parietal patient with visual extinction and spatial neglect , 2002 .

[57]  G. Vallar Extrapersonal Visual Unilateral Spatial Neglect and Its Neuroanatomy , 2001, NeuroImage.

[58]  J. Driver,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Evoked Potential Correlates of Conscious and Unconscious Vision in Parietal Extinction Patients , 2001, NeuroImage.

[59]  S. Ferber,et al.  Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe , 2001, Nature.

[60]  R. Rafal,et al.  Neural fate of seen and unseen faces in visuospatial neglect: A combined event-related functional MRI and event-related potential study , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[61]  S. Zeki,et al.  The neurology of saccades and covert shifts in spatial attention: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[62]  C. Frith,et al.  Unconscious activation of visual cortex in the damaged right hemisphere of a parietal patient with extinction. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[63]  R. Rafal,et al.  A systematic study of visual extinction. Between- and within-field deficits of attention in hemispatial neglect. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[64]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[65]  K. Heilman,et al.  Neglect and Related Disorders , 1984, Seminars in neurology.

[66]  Gregory P. Lee,et al.  Cerebral lateralization , 1999, Neurology.

[67]  S. Kiebel,et al.  Training-induced brain plasticity in aphasia. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[68]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Effects of Attention on the Processing of Motion in Macaque Middle Temporal and Medial Superior Temporal Visual Cortical Areas , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The functional anatomy of attention to visual motion. A functional MRI study. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[70]  G. Rees,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. , 1998, Science.

[71]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Parietal neglect and visual awareness , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[72]  C. Kennard,et al.  Abnormal temporal dynamics of visual attention in spatial neglect patients , 1997, Nature.

[73]  S. Kiebel,et al.  Brain Representation of Active and Passive Movements , 1996, NeuroImage.

[74]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST , 1996, Nature.

[75]  S G Lomber,et al.  Reversible visual hemineglect. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[76]  R. Woods,et al.  Recovery from wernicke's aphasia: A positron emission tomographic study , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[77]  G Vallar,et al.  Anatomical correlates of visual and tactile extinction in humans: a clinical CT scan study. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[78]  K Willmes,et al.  The case of aphasia or neglect after striatocapsular infarction. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[79]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Functional reorganization of the brain in recovery from striatocapsular infarction in man , 1992, Annals of neurology.

[80]  M M Mesulam,et al.  Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[81]  J. Baron Depression of Energy Metabolism in Distant Brain Structures: Studies with Positron Emission Tomography in Stroke Patients , 1989, Seminars in neurology.

[82]  J. Lynch,et al.  Deficits of visual attention and saccadic eye movements after lesions of parietooccipital cortex in monkeys. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[83]  J. Bogousslavsky,et al.  Subcortical neglect: Neuropsychological, SPECT, and neuropathological correlations with anterior choroidal artery territory infarction , 1988, Annals of neurology.

[84]  D. Perani,et al.  The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[85]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[86]  M. Mesulam A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.

[87]  J. Winn,et al.  Brain , 1878, The Lancet.

[88]  M. Kinsbourne,et al.  A model for the mechanism of unilateral neglect of space. , 1970, Transactions of the American Neurological Association.

[89]  Macdonald Critchley,et al.  THE PHENOMENON OF TACTILE IN ATTENTION WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO PARIETAL LESIONS , 1949 .

[90]  W. R. Brain VISUAL DISORIENTATION WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO LESIONS OF THE RIGHT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERE , 1941 .

[91]  D. Whitteridge,et al.  AKINETIC MUTISM WITH AN EPIDERMOID CYST OF THE 3RD VENTRICLE , 1941 .