When Neuroscience ‘Touches’ Architecture: From Hapticity to a Supramodal Functioning of the Human Brain

In the last decades, the rapid growth of functional brain imaging methodologies allowed cognitive neuroscience to address open questions in philosophy and social sciences. At the same time, novel insights from cognitive neuroscience research have begun to influence various disciplines, leading to a turn to cognition and emotion in the fields of planning and architectural design. Since 2003, the Academy of Neuroscience for Architecture has been supporting ‘neuro-architecture’ as a way to connect neuroscience and the study of behavioral responses to the built environment. Among the many topics related to multisensory perceptual integration and embodiment, the concept of hapticity was recently introduced, suggesting a pivotal role of tactile perception and haptic imagery in architectural appraisal. Arguments have thus risen in favor of the existence of shared cognitive foundations between hapticity and the supramodal functional architecture of the human brain. Precisely, supramodality refers to the functional feature of defined brain regions to process and represent specific information content in a more abstract way, independently of the sensory modality conveying such information to the brain. Here, we highlight some commonalities and differences between the concepts of hapticity and supramodality according to the distinctive perspectives of architecture and cognitive neuroscience. This comparison and connection between these two different approaches may lead to novel observations in regard to people–environment relationships, and even provide empirical foundations for a renewed evidence-based design theory.

[1]  R. Leys,et al.  The Turn to Affect: A Critique , 2011, Critical Inquiry.

[2]  Review of Self-Organization and the City , 2004 .

[3]  Emiliano Ricciardi,et al.  How concepts are encoded in the human brain: A modality independent, category-based cortical organization of semantic knowledge , 2016, NeuroImage.

[4]  Lotfi B. Merabet,et al.  Neural correlates associated with superior tactile symmetry perception in the early blind , 2015, Cortex.

[5]  Amir Amedi,et al.  Origins of task-specific sensory-independent organization in the visual and auditory brain: neuroscience evidence, open questions and clinical implications , 2015, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[6]  Jacob Hornik,et al.  Tactile Stimulation and Consumer Response , 1992 .

[7]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  Are visual texture-selective areas recruited during haptic texture discrimination? , 2013, NeuroImage.

[8]  S Lakatos,et al.  Haptic form perception: Relative salience of local and global features , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  C. Gross Brain, Vision, Memory: Tales in the History of Neuroscience , 1998 .

[10]  Joan Llobera,et al.  “Seeing” and “feeling” architecture: how bodily self-consciousness alters architectonic experience and affects the perception of interiors , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[11]  Seeing and Feeling. , 2003 .

[12]  Dennis Gallagher,et al.  Eye and Mind , 1980 .

[13]  Simon Lacey,et al.  Crossmodal and multisensory interactions between vision and touch , 2015, Scholarpedia.

[14]  Oshin Vartanian,et al.  Impact of contour on aesthetic judgments and approach-avoidance decisions in architecture , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  G Franz,et al.  An empirical approach to the experience of architectural space in VR - Exploring relations between features and affective appraisals of rectangular interiors , 2003, eCAADe proceedings.

[16]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[17]  M. Lange The smart city you love to hate Exploring the role of affect in hybrid urbanism , 2013 .

[18]  D. Fitzgerald,et al.  Social Science and Neuroscience beyond Interdisciplinarity: Experimental Entanglements , 2015, Theory, culture & society.

[19]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Topography of social touching depends on emotional bonds between humans , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  J W Belliveau,et al.  Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Science.

[21]  Luciano Fadiga,et al.  Beyond Motor Scheme: A Supramodal Distributed Representation in the Action-Observation Network , 2013, PloS one.

[22]  Juhani Pallasmaa,et al.  Mind in architecture : neuroscience, embodiment, and the future of design , 2015 .

[23]  Stuart Firestein,et al.  Ignorance: How it drives science , 2012 .

[24]  Anne van der Veen Complexity, Cognition and the City (Understanding Complex Systems) by Juval Portugali , 2012, J. Artif. Soc. Soc. Simul..

[25]  H. Schifferstein,et al.  Touching Materials Visually: About the Dominance of Vision in Building Material Assessment , 2013 .

[26]  S. Lilienfeld,et al.  Fifty psychological and psychiatric terms to avoid: a list of inaccurate, misleading, misused, ambiguous, and logically confused words and phrases , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[27]  F. Lepore,et al.  Crossmodal plasticity in sensory loss. , 2011, Progress in brain research.

[28]  Christian Keysers,et al.  Somatosensation in social perception , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[29]  A. Amedi,et al.  Functional imaging of human crossmodal identification and object recognition , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  George Markowsky,et al.  Misconceptions about the Golden Ratio , 1992 .

[31]  H. Mallgrave Embodiment and enculturation: the future of architectural design , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[32]  Jack L. Nasar,et al.  Neural codes of seeing architectural styles , 2016 .

[33]  Susan J. Smith,et al.  Editorial: Emotional geographies , 2001 .

[34]  V. Gallese,et al.  Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  David P. Friedman,et al.  Physiological evidence for serial processing in somatosensory cortex. , 1987, Science.

[36]  P. Pietrini,et al.  Spatial imagery relies on a sensory independent, though sensory sensitive, functional organization within the parietal cortex: A fMRI study of angle discrimination in sighted and congenitally blind individuals , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  Máire Eithne O'Neill,et al.  Corporeal Experience: A Haptic Way of Knowing , 2001 .

[38]  J. Eberhard Brain landscape : the coexistance of neuroscience and architecture , 2009 .

[39]  Emiliano Ricciardi,et al.  The blind brain: How (lack of) vision shapes the morphological and functional architecture of the human brain , 2014, Experimental biology and medicine.

[40]  P. Pietrini,et al.  Imagery and spatial processes in blindness and visual impairment , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[41]  P. Pietrini,et al.  A topographical organization for action representation in the human brain , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[42]  Luciano Fadiga,et al.  Do We Really Need Vision? How Blind People “See” the Actions of Others , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  C. Adams ‘Nature and I are Two’ , 2001 .

[44]  R. Martyn Bracewell,et al.  Brain Mechanisms of Haptic Perception , 2008, The Sense of Touch and its Rendering.

[45]  Oshin Vartanian,et al.  Architectural design and the brain: effects of ceiling height and perceived enclosure on beauty judgments and approach-avoidance decisions , 2015 .

[46]  T. Hendler,et al.  Visuo-haptic object-related activation in the ventral visual pathway , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  M. Guazzelli,et al.  Tactile spatial working memory activates the dorsal extrastriate cortical pathway in congenitally blind individuals. , 2008, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[48]  Holger H Ge FECHNER'S EXPERIMENTAL AESTHETICS AND THE GOLDEN SECTION HYPOTHESIS TODAY , 1995 .

[49]  Roberta L Klatzky,et al.  Haptic object perception: spatial dimensionality and relation to vision , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[50]  N. Weisz,et al.  Revisiting the adaptive and maladaptive effects of crossmodal plasticity , 2014, Neuroscience.

[51]  T. Hendler,et al.  Convergence of visual and tactile shape processing in the human lateral occipital complex. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[52]  T. Vecchi,et al.  Supramodality effects in visual and haptic spatial processes. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[53]  Pietro Pietrini,et al.  Blindness and Consciousness: New Light from the Dark , 2009 .

[54]  M. Goodale One brain - two visual systems , 2006 .

[55]  Matteo Bianchi,et al.  A synergy-based hand control is encoded in human motor cortical areas , 2016, eLife.

[56]  H. Höge Fechner's Experimental Aesthetics and the Golden Section Hypothesis Today , 1995 .

[57]  P. Pietrini,et al.  Mind the blind brain to understand the sighted one! Is there a supramodal cortical functional architecture? , 2014, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[58]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  How we experience immersive virtual environments: the concept of presence and its measurement * , 2009 .

[59]  P. Pietrini,et al.  Neural correlates of spatial working memory in humans: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study comparing visual and tactile processes , 2006, Neuroscience.

[60]  Edward Hall,et al.  The hidden dimension: an anthropologist examines man's use of space in public and private , 1969 .

[61]  Charles Willard Moore,et al.  Body, Memory, and Architecture , 1977 .

[62]  J. Portugali Toward a Cognitive Approach to Urban Dynamics , 2004 .

[63]  P. Pietrini,et al.  New light from the dark: what blindness can teach us about brain function. , 2011, Current opinion in neurology.

[64]  Ute Hoffmann,et al.  The Eyes of the Skin , 2024 .

[65]  H. Mallgrave The Architect's Brain: Neuroscience, Creativity, and Architecture , 2009 .

[66]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[67]  Clement Falbo,et al.  The Golden Ratio—A Contrary Viewpoint , 2005 .

[68]  A. Block,et al.  ‘Nature and I are Two’: A Critical Examination of the Biophilia Hypothesis , 2011 .

[69]  K. Kreij Sensory intensification in architecture , 2008 .

[70]  Emiliano Ricciardi,et al.  Beyond sensory images: Object-based representation in the human ventral pathway. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[71]  J. Haxby,et al.  The effect of visual experience on the development of functional architecture in hMT+. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[72]  Emiliano Ricciardi,et al.  Congenital blindness affects diencephalic but not mesencephalic structures in the human brain , 2015, Brain Structure and Function.

[73]  J. Portugali Complexity, Cognition and the City , 2011 .

[74]  Giovanni Cioni,et al.  Massage Accelerates Brain Development and the Maturation of Visual Function , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.