Independence and Cooperation in Research. The Motivations and Costs of Collaboration.

Freedom and independence are strong precepts in science and scholarship [12, 13, 21, 32]. Scientists are expected to select problems freely, to exercise independent judgment in methods and techniques, and to evaluate without restraint the validity of their own results and those of others [12]. As a correlate to these structural norms, scholarship tends to attract the "solitary mind" [6]. Biographical and clinical studies show consistently that productive scientists are independent, self-sufficient, and self-directed. In an early study, Ann Roe concluded that "the most important single factor in the making of a scientist [is] the need and ability to develop personal independence to a high degree" [29, p. 25]. This autonomy is exhibited early in scientists' lives and is apparent in their preference for teachers who let them alone and in their dispositions toward interpersonal relations [29]. As adults, productive scientists tend to be detached from their immediate families, isolated from social relations, and attached to the objects and abstract ideas of their work [4, 30, 33]. Productive scientists emerge as "strongly motivated, dominant person[s] who are not overly concerned with other persons' lives or with obtaining approval for the work Ithev arel doing" 1[4 n. 12041.

[1]  R. Merton Social Theory and Social Structure , 1958 .

[2]  Anne Roe,et al.  A Psychologist Examines 64 Eminent Scientists , 1952 .

[3]  F. M. Andrews,et al.  Creativity and the scientist , 1962 .

[4]  T. Kuhn,et al.  The Structure of Scientific Revolutions , 1963 .

[5]  J. Chambers,et al.  Creative Scientists of Today , 1964, Science.

[6]  W. Hagstrom,et al.  Traditional and Modern Forms of Scientific Teamwork , 1964 .

[7]  D. Price Little Science, Big Science , 1965 .

[8]  W. Hagstrom The scientific community , 1966 .

[9]  R. Lindsay,et al.  The social system of science , 1966 .

[10]  Calvin W. Taylor,et al.  Biographical Predictors of Scientific Performance , 1967, Science.

[11]  Harriet Zuckerman,et al.  Age, aging, and age structure in science , 1968 .

[12]  R. Merton The Matthew Effect in Science , 1968, Science.

[13]  A. A. Manten Statistical analysis of a scientific discipline: palynology , 1970 .

[14]  Woman's Place: Options and Limits in Professional Careers. , 1971 .

[15]  Stephen Cole,et al.  Social Stratification in Science. , 1973 .

[16]  A. J. Meadows Communication in science , 1974 .

[17]  J. Centra,et al.  Women, Men and the Doctorate. , 1974 .

[18]  B. Reskin Sex Differentiation and the Social Organization of Science , 1978 .

[19]  Lowell L. Hargens Relations Between Work Habits, Research Technologies, and Eminence in Science , 1978 .

[20]  A. Heffner Authorship Recognition of Subordinates in Collaborative Research , 1979 .

[21]  Stanley Presser,et al.  Collaboration and the Quality of Research , 1980 .

[22]  Paul D. Allison Processes of stratification in science , 1980 .

[23]  Climbing the Academic Ladder: Doctoral Women Scientists in Academe , 1980 .

[24]  Sharlene Hesse-Biber,et al.  Women at Work , 1985 .