Shared Values, Social Trust, and the Perception of Geographic Cancer Clusters

Extensive scientific investigations often fail to identify specific carcinogens that have caused geographic clusters of cancer cases. In many such examples, public health officials and other experts have concluded that the cluster is not the result of a particular local environmental condition. Despite this conclusion by experts, concerned members of local communities often persist in believing that the cancer cluster was not random. The present study accounts for the persistence of this belief on the basis of two factors: (a) the tendency of the human mind to identify patterns (and causes), rather than randomness; and (b) a lack of social trust in public health experts. It was expected that perceived shared values evoke social trust. Individuals who conclude that public health experts share their values should be more likely to accept the experts’ conclusion that a cancer cluster reflects randomness, not a particular local cause. Individuals who trust authorities should be more inclined than individuals not having trust to accept that a geographic cluster of cancer cases is a coincidence. Data from Swiss students (N= 334) supported these expectations. Additionally, significant gender differences were observed. Females had less trust in authorities and perceived the cancer cluster as less likely to be a result of pure chance than did males. Practical implications of the results are discussed.

[1]  M. Siegrist,et al.  Perception of Hazards: The Role of Social Trust and Knowledge , 2000, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[2]  Craig W. Trumbo,et al.  Public requests for cancer cluster investigations: a survey of state health departments. , 2000, American journal of public health.

[3]  M. Siegrist,et al.  Salient Value Similarity, Social Trust, and Risk/Benefit Perception , 2000, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[4]  M. Siegrist The Influence of Trust and Perceptions of Risks and Benefits on the Acceptance of Gene Technology , 2000, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[5]  Michael Siegrist,et al.  A Causal Model Explaining the Perception and Acceptance of Gene Technology1 , 1999 .

[6]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Not So Different After All: A Cross-Discipline View Of Trust , 1998 .

[7]  Clifford Konold,et al.  Making Sense of Randomness " Implicit Encoding as a Basis for Judgment , 1997 .

[8]  Paul Slovic,et al.  Perception of Risk From Radiation , 1996 .

[9]  Jonathan Harr,et al.  A Civil Action , 1995 .

[10]  Timothy C. Earle,et al.  Social Trust , 1995 .

[11]  Peter M. Bentler,et al.  Estimates and tests in structural equation modeling. , 1995 .

[12]  Paul Slovic,et al.  Perceived risk, trust, and democracy , 1993 .

[13]  Paul Slovic,et al.  Trust as a Determinant of Opposition to a High‐Level Radioactive Waste Repository: Analysis of a Structural Model , 1992 .

[14]  Robert E. O'Connor,et al.  Determinants of Risk Perceptions of a Hazardous Waste Site , 1992 .

[15]  Peter M. Bentler,et al.  EQS : structural equations program manual , 1989 .

[16]  Peter M. Sandman,et al.  Public Response to the Risk from Geological Radon , 1987 .

[17]  A. Tversky,et al.  The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences , 1985, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  Laura M. Davidson,et al.  Natural Disaster and Technological Catastrophe , 1983 .

[19]  A. Tversky,et al.  BELIEF IN THE LAW OF SMALL NUMBERS , 1971, Pediatrics.

[20]  S. Streufert Trust. A Mechanism for the Reduction of Social Complexity , 1968 .