Role of Self-Generated Odor Cues in Contextual Representation

As first demonstrated in the patient H.M., the hippocampus is critically involved in forming episodic memories, the recall of “what” happened “where” and “when.” In rodents, the clearest functional correlate of hippocampal primary neurons is the place field: a cell fires predominantly when the animal is in a specific part of the environment, typically defined relative to the available visuospatial cues. However, rodents have relatively poor visual acuity. Furthermore, they are highly adept at navigating in total darkness. This raises the question of how other sensory modalities might contribute to a hippocampal representation of an environment. Rodents have a highly developed olfactory system, suggesting that cues such as odor trails may be important. To test this, we familiarized mice to a visually cued environment over a number of days while maintaining odor cues. During familiarization, self‐generated odor cues unique to each animal were collected by re‐using absorbent paperboard flooring from one session to the next. Visual and odor cues were then put in conflict by counter‐rotating the recording arena and the flooring. Perhaps surprisingly, place fields seemed to follow the visual cue rotation exclusively, raising the question of whether olfactory cues have any influence at all on a hippocampal spatial representation. However, subsequent removal of the familiar, self‐generated odor cues severely disrupted both long‐term stability and rotation to visual cues in a novel environment. Our data suggest that odor cues, in the absence of additional rule learning, do not provide a discriminative spatial signal that anchors place fields. Such cues do, however, become integral to the context over time and exert a powerful influence on the stability of its hippocampal representation. © 2014 The Authors. Hippocampus Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

[1]  R. Douglas,et al.  Characterization of mouse cortical spatial vision , 2004, Vision Research.

[2]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Tetrodes markedly improve the reliability and yield of multiple single-unit isolation from multi-unit recordings in cat striate cortex , 1995, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[3]  R. C. Honey,et al.  Acquired equivalence and distinctiveness of cues. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[4]  J. O’Keefe Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat , 1976, Experimental Neurology.

[5]  P. E. Gold,et al.  Acetylcholine release in the hippocampus and prelimbic cortex during acquisition of a socially transmitted food preference , 2011, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[6]  R. Muller,et al.  The effects of changes in the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex-spike cells , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[8]  P. Best,et al.  Place cells and silent cells in the hippocampus of freely-behaving rats , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  J. B. Ranck,et al.  Localization and anatomical identification of theta and complex spike cells in dorsal hippocampal formation of rats , 1975, Experimental Neurology.

[10]  C. Carter,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: An update of theory and data , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  E. Bostock,et al.  Experience‐dependent modifications of hippocampal place cell firing , 1991, Hippocampus.

[12]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Muller,et al.  The hippocampus as a cognitive graph (abridged version) , 1991, Hippocampus.

[14]  R. Hampson,et al.  Hippocampal place cells: stereotypy and plasticity , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Cues that hippocampal place cells encode: Dynamic and hierarchical representation of local and distal stimuli , 1997, Hippocampus.

[16]  L. Irwin,et al.  Novel odors affect gene expression for cytokines and proteinases in the rat amygdala and hippocampus , 2012, Brain Research.

[17]  J. O’Neill,et al.  The reorganization and reactivation of hippocampal maps predict spatial memory performance , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  R. Muller,et al.  Spatial firing properties of hippocampal theta cells , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  E. Save,et al.  Contribution of multiple sensory information to place field stability in hippocampal place cells , 2000, Hippocampus.

[20]  W E Skaggs,et al.  Interactions between location and task affect the spatial and directional firing of hippocampal neurons , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[21]  Andrew P Maurer,et al.  The influence of objects on place field expression and size in distal hippocampal CA1 , 2011, Hippocampus.

[22]  Joseph D. Monaco,et al.  Attention Enhances the Retrieval and Stability of Visuospatial and Olfactory Representations in the Dorsal Hippocampus , 2009, PLoS biology.

[23]  H Eichenbaum,et al.  Using olfaction to study memory. , 1998, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  P. Lavenex,et al.  Olfactory traces and spatial learning in rats , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  E. Save,et al.  Flexible use of proximal objects and distal cues by hippocampal place cells , 2007, Hippocampus.

[26]  Douglas G Wallace,et al.  Quantification of a single exploratory trip reveals hippocampal formation mediated dead reckoning , 2002, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[27]  H. Lehn,et al.  Hippocampal involvement in retrieval of odor vs. object memories , 2013, Hippocampus.

[28]  Dorothy Tse,et al.  Schema-Dependent Gene Activation and Memory Encoding in Neocortex , 2011, Science.

[29]  M. Moser,et al.  Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping , 2008, Trends in Neurosciences.

[30]  R U Muller,et al.  Variable place-cell coupling to a continuously viewed stimulus: evidence that the hippocampus acts as a perceptual system. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[31]  David C Rowland,et al.  Neural Correlates of Long-Term Object Memory in the Mouse Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  Tom Hartley,et al.  What can the hippocampal representation of environmental geometry tell us about Hebbian learning? , 2002, Biological Cybernetics.

[34]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Spatial information content and reliability of hippocampal CA1 neurons: Effects of visual input , 1994, Hippocampus.

[35]  Jeounghoon Kim,et al.  Multiple cognitive control mechanisms associated with the nature of conflict , 2010, Neuroscience Letters.

[36]  E. Kandel,et al.  Increased Attention to Spatial Context Increases Both Place Field Stability and Spatial Memory , 2004, Neuron.

[37]  Howard Eichenbauma Using Olfaction to Study Memory , 1998 .

[38]  P. Lavenex,et al.  Influence of local environmental olfactory cues on place learning in rats , 1995, Physiology & Behavior.

[39]  R. E. Brown,et al.  Visual detection, pattern discrimination and visual acuity in 14 strains of mice , 2006, Genes, brain, and behavior.

[40]  Kathryn J Jeffery,et al.  Heterogeneous Modulation of Place Cell Firing by Changes in Context , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  K. Deisseroth,et al.  Dynamics of Retrieval Strategies for Remote Memories , 2011, Cell.