How do animals get about by vision? Visually controlled locomotion and orientation after 50 years.

James Gibson’s 1958 paper, “Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals,” was a milepost in his development of a theory of perception that would do justice to ordinary behavior in natural environments. In particular, it provided the first statement of an information-based approach to perception and action, which continues to influence much contemporary work in humans, animals, and robots. In this comment, I discuss the context for Gibson’s evolving ideas about perception and offer a brief status report on his theory of visual control.

[1]  J. Lackner,et al.  Coupling of fingertip somatosensory information to head and body sway , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Behavioral dynamics of steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. , 2003 .

[3]  David N. Lee Visual proprioceptive control of stance , 1975 .

[4]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow , 1998, Current Biology.

[5]  W H Warren,et al.  Visual control of braking: a test of the tau hypothesis. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  Cynthia F Moss,et al.  Echolocating Bats Use a Nearly Time-Optimal Strategy to Intercept Prey , 2006, PLoS biology.

[7]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Visual Guidance of Intercepting a Moving Target on Foot , 2004, Perception.

[8]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Calibration, information, and control strategies for braking to avoid a collision. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  David N. Lee,et al.  A Theory of Visual Control of Braking Based on Information about Time-to-Collision , 1976, Perception.

[10]  A. Duchon,et al.  A Visual Equalization Strategy for Locomotor Control: Of Honeybees, Robots, and Humans , 2002, Psychological science.

[11]  David N. Lee,et al.  Visual proprioceptive control of standing in human infants , 1974 .

[12]  R. Hetherington The Perception of the Visual World , 1952 .

[13]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[14]  M T Turvey,et al.  Optical information about the severity of upcoming contacts. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Benoît G. Bardy,et al.  Postural Responses to Two Technologies for Generating Optical Flow , 2004, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[16]  C. C. A. M. Gielen,et al.  Postural adjustments induced by simulated motion of differently structured environments , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Optic flow and scene structure do not always contribute to the control of human walking , 2002, Vision Research.

[18]  M. Grealy,et al.  Closing the Gap : The Scientific Writings of David N. Lee , 2007 .

[19]  R. Olberg,et al.  Prey pursuit and interception in dragonflies , 2000, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[20]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[21]  T. Brandt,et al.  Differential effects of retinal target displacement, changing size and changing disparity in the control of anterior/posterior and lateral body sway , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Geert J P Savelsbergh,et al.  Rate of Change of Angular Bearing as the Relevant Property in a Horizontal Interception Task During Locomotion , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[23]  K. Adolph Learning to learn in the development of action , 2005 .

[24]  W. Warren,et al.  Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  G. Montagne,et al.  A Robust Solution for Dealing With Environmental Changes in Intercepting Moving Balls , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[26]  Benoît G. Bardy,et al.  Motion parallax is used to control postural sway during walking , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Second-order optic flow , 1992 .

[28]  J. Todd,et al.  The perception of surface curvature from optical motion , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  B. Guschlbauer,et al.  The significance of proprioception on postural stabilization as assessed by ischemia , 1984, Brain Research.

[30]  Martin A. Giese,et al.  Frequency dependence of the action-perception cycle for postural control in a moving visual environment: relative phase dynamics , 1994, Biological Cybernetics.

[31]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[32]  Wendy D. Zosh,et al.  Optic Flow Drives Human Visuo-Locomotor Adaptation , 2007, Current Biology.

[33]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Range perception through apparent image speed in freely flying honeybees , 1991, Visual Neuroscience.

[34]  K. Shapiro,et al.  The contingent negative variation (CNV) event-related potential (ERP) predicts the attentional blink , 2008 .

[35]  W. D. Craft,et al.  Foundations of spatial vision: from retinal images to perceived shapes. , 2000, Psychological review.

[36]  J. Gibson Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. , 1998, British journal of psychology.

[37]  William H. Warren,et al.  Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  James J. Gibson,et al.  Reprinted from The British Journal of Psychology (1958), 49, 182-194: Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. , 2009, British journal of psychology.

[39]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Behavioral Dynamics of Visually Guided Locomotion , 2008 .

[40]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Behavioral dynamics of avoiding a moving obstacle , 2010 .

[41]  Brett R Fajen Learning novel mappings from optic flow to the control of action. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[42]  W. H. Warren,et al.  Behavioral dynamics of intercepting a moving target , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  L. S. Mark,et al.  Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: a study of sitting and stair climbing. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.