The relationship between P3 and neuropsychological function in an adult life span sample

The relationship of P3 to age and neuropsychological performance was investigated in a sample of 71 well-functioning adults ranging in age from 21.8 to 94.7 years. ERPs were recorded while the participants performed an auditory two-stimuli oddball task in which the rare tones were to be counted. The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) and the digit span subtest from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-R (WAIS-R) were administered. Regression analyses showed significant, linear effects of age on P3 latency and amplitude. Significant relationships between P3 and neuropsychological measures were found, in that P3 latency correlated moderately in predictable ways with scores on matrices, block design, and digit span. Overall, these relationships are best characterized by a linear function, but a non-linear component is involved in the relationship between P3 latency and fluid tests. Finally, a linear relationship between ERP components and age was found, while a curvilinear relationship was found between age and block design and matrices, respectively. There appears to be either partially different functions or structures underlying performance on these tests, the P3 component and performance on neuropsychological tests, or one must assume some variant of a multiplicative, as opposed to an additive, model of cognition.

[1]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[2]  M. Fabiani,et al.  Individual differences in P3 scalp distribution in older adults, and their relationship to frontal lobe function. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[3]  F E Bloom,et al.  P300 assessment of early Alzheimer's disease. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[4]  A. Kok On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[5]  D. Kausler Experimental psychology, cognition, and human aging , 1982 .

[6]  Michael G. H. Coles,et al.  Mental chronometry and the study of human information processing. , 1995 .

[7]  Margot J. Taylor,et al.  Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[8]  E. Donchin Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[9]  Vincent Egan,et al.  Size isn't everything: A study of brain volume, intelligence and auditory evoked potentials , 1994 .

[10]  H. Michalewski,et al.  A comparison of the emitted late positive potential in older and young adults. , 1982, Journal of gerontology.

[11]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 and individual differences: morning/evening activity preference, food, and time-of-day. , 1992, Psychophysiology.

[12]  K. Walhovd,et al.  Two- and three-stimuli auditory oddball ERP tasks and neuropsychological measures in aging , 2001, Neuroreport.

[13]  Anders M. Fjell,et al.  P300 and Neuropsychological Tests as Measures of Aging: Scalp Topography and Cognitive Changes , 2004, Brain Topography.

[14]  T W Picton,et al.  The effects of age on human event-related potentials. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[15]  Diana S. Woodruff-Pak,et al.  The Neuropsychology of Aging , 1997 .

[16]  D Friedman,et al.  An overview of age-related changes in the scalp distribution of P3b. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[17]  Denise C. Park,et al.  The basic mechanisms accounting for age-related decline in cognitive function. , 2000 .

[18]  M. Fabiani,et al.  Changes in brain activity patterns in aging: the novelty oddball. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[19]  Denise C. Park,et al.  Cognitive Aging: A Primer , 1999 .

[20]  D. Papalia Adult Development and Aging , 1995 .

[21]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Wet Mind: The New Cognitive Neuroscience , 1995 .

[22]  Marie-France Ehrlich,et al.  Working memory and aging: Deficit or strategy differences? , 1997 .

[23]  A. Starr,et al.  P300 latency correlates with digit span. , 1983, Psychophysiology.

[24]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Effects of aging on event-related brain potentials and reaction times in an auditory oddball task. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[25]  H. Shibasaki,et al.  Human auditory and somatosensory event-related potentials: effects of response condition and age. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[26]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 in young and elderly subjects: auditory frequency and intensity effects. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[27]  B. O’Donnell,et al.  Active and passive P3 latency and psychometric performance: influence of age and individual differences. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[28]  Denise C. Park,et al.  Handbook of the Psychology of Aging , 1979 .

[29]  J. Polich,et al.  Meta-analysis of P300 normative aging studies. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[30]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  Clinical utility of long latency 'cognitive' event-related potentials (P3): the cons. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[31]  K. Squires,et al.  Age-related variations in evoked potentials to auditory stimuli in normal human subjects. , 1978, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[32]  I. Reinvang Validation of reaction time in continuous performance tasks as an index of attention by electrophysiological measures. , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[33]  R. Hull Hearing in Aging , 1995 .

[34]  Norbert Jaušovec,et al.  Correlations between ERP parameters and intelligence: a reconsideration , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[35]  Jacques Grégoire,et al.  Effect of age on forward and backward digit spans , 1997 .

[36]  J. Birren,et al.  History, concepts, and theory in the psychology of aging , 1996 .

[37]  I. Reinvang Cognitive Event-Related Potentials in Neuropsychological Assessment , 1999, Neuropsychology Review.

[38]  T. Salthouse Aging and measures of processing speed , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[39]  R. L. Babcock,et al.  An examination of the adult age-related differences on the raven's advanced progressive matrices: A structural equations approach , 1996 .

[40]  M. Rugg,et al.  Electrophysiology of Mind. , 1996 .

[41]  J. Cerella Information processing rates in the elderly. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.

[42]  J. T. Marsh,et al.  Exponential electrophysiological aging: P3 latency. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[43]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  Clinical application of the P3 component of event-related potentials. I. Normal aging. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[44]  L D Blumhardt,et al.  Wave form variations in auditory event-related potentials evoked by a memory-scanning task and their relationship with tests of intellectual function. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[45]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  Multichannel auditory event-related brain potentials: effects of normal aging on the scalp distribution of N1, P2, N2 and P300 latencies and amplitudes. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[46]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.