The relationship between P3 and neuropsychological function in an adult life span sample
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[2] M. Fabiani,et al. Individual differences in P3 scalp distribution in older adults, and their relationship to frontal lobe function. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[3] F E Bloom,et al. P300 assessment of early Alzheimer's disease. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[4] A. Kok. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[5] D. Kausler. Experimental psychology, cognition, and human aging , 1982 .
[6] Michael G. H. Coles,et al. Mental chronometry and the study of human information processing. , 1995 .
[7] Margot J. Taylor,et al. Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[8] E. Donchin. Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.
[9] Vincent Egan,et al. Size isn't everything: A study of brain volume, intelligence and auditory evoked potentials , 1994 .
[10] H. Michalewski,et al. A comparison of the emitted late positive potential in older and young adults. , 1982, Journal of gerontology.
[11] J. Polich,et al. P300 and individual differences: morning/evening activity preference, food, and time-of-day. , 1992, Psychophysiology.
[12] K. Walhovd,et al. Two- and three-stimuli auditory oddball ERP tasks and neuropsychological measures in aging , 2001, Neuroreport.
[13] Anders M. Fjell,et al. P300 and Neuropsychological Tests as Measures of Aging: Scalp Topography and Cognitive Changes , 2004, Brain Topography.
[14] T W Picton,et al. The effects of age on human event-related potentials. , 1984, Psychophysiology.
[15] Diana S. Woodruff-Pak,et al. The Neuropsychology of Aging , 1997 .
[16] D Friedman,et al. An overview of age-related changes in the scalp distribution of P3b. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[17] Denise C. Park,et al. The basic mechanisms accounting for age-related decline in cognitive function. , 2000 .
[18] M. Fabiani,et al. Changes in brain activity patterns in aging: the novelty oddball. , 1995, Psychophysiology.
[19] Denise C. Park,et al. Cognitive Aging: A Primer , 1999 .
[20] D. Papalia. Adult Development and Aging , 1995 .
[21] S. Kosslyn,et al. Wet Mind: The New Cognitive Neuroscience , 1995 .
[22] Marie-France Ehrlich,et al. Working memory and aging: Deficit or strategy differences? , 1997 .
[23] A. Starr,et al. P300 latency correlates with digit span. , 1983, Psychophysiology.
[24] S A Hillyard,et al. Effects of aging on event-related brain potentials and reaction times in an auditory oddball task. , 2007, Psychophysiology.
[25] H. Shibasaki,et al. Human auditory and somatosensory event-related potentials: effects of response condition and age. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[26] J. Polich,et al. P300 in young and elderly subjects: auditory frequency and intensity effects. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[27] B. O’Donnell,et al. Active and passive P3 latency and psychometric performance: influence of age and individual differences. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[28] Denise C. Park,et al. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging , 1979 .
[29] J. Polich,et al. Meta-analysis of P300 normative aging studies. , 1996, Psychophysiology.
[30] A Pfefferbaum,et al. Clinical utility of long latency 'cognitive' event-related potentials (P3): the cons. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[31] K. Squires,et al. Age-related variations in evoked potentials to auditory stimuli in normal human subjects. , 1978, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[32] I. Reinvang. Validation of reaction time in continuous performance tasks as an index of attention by electrophysiological measures. , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.
[33] R. Hull. Hearing in Aging , 1995 .
[34] Norbert Jaušovec,et al. Correlations between ERP parameters and intelligence: a reconsideration , 2000, Biological Psychology.
[35] Jacques Grégoire,et al. Effect of age on forward and backward digit spans , 1997 .
[36] J. Birren,et al. History, concepts, and theory in the psychology of aging , 1996 .
[37] I. Reinvang. Cognitive Event-Related Potentials in Neuropsychological Assessment , 1999, Neuropsychology Review.
[38] T. Salthouse. Aging and measures of processing speed , 2000, Biological Psychology.
[39] R. L. Babcock,et al. An examination of the adult age-related differences on the raven's advanced progressive matrices: A structural equations approach , 1996 .
[40] M. Rugg,et al. Electrophysiology of Mind. , 1996 .
[41] J. Cerella. Information processing rates in the elderly. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.
[42] J. T. Marsh,et al. Exponential electrophysiological aging: P3 latency. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[43] A Pfefferbaum,et al. Clinical application of the P3 component of event-related potentials. I. Normal aging. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[44] L D Blumhardt,et al. Wave form variations in auditory event-related potentials evoked by a memory-scanning task and their relationship with tests of intellectual function. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[45] H. Semlitsch,et al. Multichannel auditory event-related brain potentials: effects of normal aging on the scalp distribution of N1, P2, N2 and P300 latencies and amplitudes. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[46] H. Semlitsch,et al. A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.