Environmental Consistency Modulation of Error Sensitivity During Motor Adaptation is Explicitly Controlled
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. Karniel,et al. Target size matters: target errors contribute to the generalization of implicit visuomotor learning. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.
[2] J. Krakauer,et al. Generalization of Motor Learning Depends on the History of Prior Action , 2006, PLoS biology.
[3] G. Hammond,et al. Impaired savings despite intact initial learning of motor adaptation in Parkinson’s disease , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[4] J. O. de Xivry,et al. Internal model recalibration does not deteriorate with age while motor adaptation does , 2018, bioRxiv.
[5] John W. Krakauer,et al. Motor Learning: The Great Rate Debate , 2014, Current Biology.
[6] A. Haith,et al. Movement Repetition Facilitates Response Preparation. , 2018, Cell reports.
[7] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Flexible Cognitive Strategies during Motor Learning , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[8] R. Shadmehr,et al. Modulation of error-sensitivity during a prism adaptation task in people with cerebellar degeneration. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[9] Sarah A Hutter,et al. Relative sensitivity of explicit re-aiming and implicit motor adaptation , 2018, bioRxiv.
[10] J J Bloomberg,et al. Neural correlates of multi-day learning and savings in sensorimotor adaptation , 2018, Scientific Reports.
[11] Matthew J. Crossley,et al. Savings upon Re-Aiming in Visuomotor Adaptation , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[12] David M. Huberdeau,et al. The Influence of Movement Preparation Time on the Expression of Visuomotor Learning and Savings , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[13] John W. Krakauer,et al. Practice induces a qualitative change in the memory representation for visuomotor learning , 2017 .
[14] Hiroshi Imamizu,et al. Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool , 2000, Nature.
[15] J. Krakauer,et al. Explicit and Implicit Contributions to Learning in a Sensorimotor Adaptation Task , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[16] P. Haggard,et al. Action and awareness in pointing tasks , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] Vincent S. Huang,et al. Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models , 2011, Neuron.
[18] R. Shadmehr,et al. Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.
[19] D. Wolpert,et al. Motor learning decline with age is related to differences in the explicit memory system , 2018, bioRxiv.
[20] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Characteristics of Implicit Sensorimotor Adaptation Revealed by Task-irrelevant Clamped Feedback , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[21] Jordan A Taylor,et al. Dissociable cognitive strategies for sensorimotor learning , 2019, Nature Communications.
[22] R. Ivry,et al. Delayed feedback during sensorimotor learning selectively disrupts adaptation but not strategy use. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.
[23] E. Bizzi,et al. Neuronal Correlates of Motor Performance and Motor Learning in the Primary Motor Cortex of Monkeys Adapting to an External Force Field , 2001, Neuron.
[24] J. Krakauer,et al. An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[25] Reza Shadmehr,et al. A memory of errors in sensorimotor learning , 2014, Science.
[26] John E. Schlerf,et al. Encoding of Sensory Prediction Errors in the Human Cerebellum , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[27] Reza Moazzezi,et al. Invariant errors reveal limitations in motor correction rather than constraints on error sensitivity , 2017, Communications Biology.
[28] H. Imamizu,et al. Neural Substrates Related to Motor Memory with Multiple Timescales in Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2015, PLoS biology.
[29] Kurt A. Thoroughman,et al. Trial-by-trial transformation of error into sensorimotor adaptation changes with environmental dynamics. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[30] Mollie K. Marko,et al. Sensitivity to prediction error in reach adaptation. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.
[31] R A Scheidt,et al. Persistence of motor adaptation during constrained, multi-joint, arm movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[32] Valeria Della-Maggiore,et al. One Week of Motor Adaptation Induces Structural Changes in Primary Motor Cortex That Predict Long-Term Memory One Year Later , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[33] P. Celnik,et al. Dissociating the roles of the cerebellum and motor cortex during adaptive learning: the motor cortex retains what the cerebellum learns. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.
[34] A. Haith,et al. Model-based and model-free mechanisms of human motor learning. , 2013, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.
[35] David M. Huberdeau,et al. Formation of a long-term memory for visuomotor adaptation following only a few trials of practice. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[36] J. Krakauer,et al. How is a motor skill learned? Change and invariance at the levels of task success and trajectory control. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.
[37] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Taking Aim at the Cognitive Side of Learning in Sensorimotor Adaptation Tasks , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[38] S. Kitazawa,et al. Effects of delayed visual information on the rate and amount of prism adaptation in the human , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[39] R. Ivry,et al. An Explicit Strategy Prevails When the Cerebellum Fails to Compute Movement Errors , 2010, The Cerebellum.
[40] Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al. Size of error affects cerebellar contributions to motor learning. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[41] Darius E. Parvin,et al. Credit Assignment in a Motor Decision Making Task Is Influenced by Agency and Not Sensory Prediction Errors , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[42] Stephan Riek,et al. Savings for visuomotor adaptation require prior history of error, not prior repetition of successful actions. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.
[43] Krista M Bond,et al. Flexible explicit but rigid implicit learning in a visuomotor adaptation task. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[44] Ferdinando A. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Representing delayed force feedback as a combination of current and delayed states. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.
[45] Robert B. Welch,et al. Adaptation to prism-displaced vision: The importance of target-pointing , 1969 .
[46] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Relevance of error: what drives motor adaptation? , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[47] Peter W. McOwan,et al. Motion integration fields are dynamically elongated in the direction of motion , 2010 .
[48] R C Miall,et al. System Identification Applied to a Visuomotor Task: Near-Optimal Human Performance in a Noisy Changing Task , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[49] M. Ernst,et al. The statistical determinants of adaptation rate in human reaching. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[50] Raymond J. Delnicki,et al. Overcoming Motor “Forgetting” Through Reinforcement Of Learned Actions , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.