A critical period for right hemisphere recruitment in American Sign Language processing

Signed languages such as American Sign Language (ASL) are natural languages that are formally similar to spoken languages, and thus present an opportunity to examine the effects of language structure and modality on the neural organization for language. Native learners of spoken languages show predominantly left-lateralized patterns of neural activation for language processing, whereas native learners of ASL show extensive right hemisphere (RH) and LH activation. We demonstrate that the RH angular gyrus is active during ASL processing only in native signers (hearing, ASL-English bilinguals) but not in those who acquired ASL after puberty (hearing, native English speakers). This is the first demonstration of a 'sensitive' or 'critical' period for language in an RH structure. This has implications for language acquisition and for understanding age-related changes in neuroplasticity more generally.

[1]  Elissa L. Newport,et al.  Maturational Constraints on Language Learning , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[2]  S. Oyama A sensitive period for the acquisition of a nonnative phonological system , 1976 .

[3]  V Bettinardi,et al.  The bilingual brain. Proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  E. Lenneberg Biological Foundations of Language , 1967 .

[5]  U. Bellugi,et al.  Neural Systems Mediating American Sign Language: Effects of Sensory Experience and Age of Acquisition , 1997, Brain and Language.

[6]  J P Rauschecker,et al.  Hemispheric specialization for English and ASL: left invariance‐right variability , 1998, Neuroreport.

[7]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  The neurobiology of sign language and its implications for the neural basis of language , 1996, Nature.

[8]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Distribution of cortical neural networks involved in word comprehension and word retrieval. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  D Bavelier,et al.  Cerebral organization for language in deaf and hearing subjects: biological constraints and effects of experience. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  F. Fazio,et al.  Brain processing of native and foreign languages , 1996, NeuroImage.

[11]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Discourse Deficits Following Right Hemisphere Damage in Deaf Signers , 1999, Brain and Language.

[12]  A. Friederici,et al.  Auditory Language Comprehension: An Event-Related fMRI Study on the Processing of Syntactic and Lexical Information , 2000, Brain and Language.

[13]  J. Risberg,et al.  Regional Cerebral Blood Flow in Sign Language Users , 1993, Brain and Language.

[14]  M. Chee,et al.  Mandarin and English Single Word Processing Studied with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Patrick Coppens,et al.  Aphasia in atypical populations , 2000 .

[16]  E. Procyk,et al.  Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  J. S. Johnson,et al.  Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  S. Curtiss Genie: A Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern-Day "Wild Child" , 1977 .

[19]  D Klein,et al.  Cerebral organization in bilinguals: a PET study of Chinese-English verb generation. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[20]  A. Galaburda,et al.  Human Cerebral Cortex: Localization, Parcellation, and Morphometry with Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  E. Klima The signs of language , 1979 .

[22]  E C Wong,et al.  Processing strategies for time‐course data sets in functional mri of the human brain , 1993, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[23]  Joy Hirsch,et al.  Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages , 1997, Nature.

[24]  J. Decety,et al.  The effects of learning and intention on the neural network involved in the perception of meaningless actions. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. , 1992, Science.

[26]  H A Whitaker,et al.  The bilingual brain. , 1978, Archives of neurology.

[27]  R. Turner,et al.  Functional mapping of the human visual cortex at 4 and 1.5 tesla using deoxygenation contrast EPI , 1993, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[28]  J. Stiles-Davis,et al.  Spatial cognition : brain bases and development , 1988 .

[29]  G. Yeni-Komshian,et al.  Age Constraints on Second-Language Acquisition , 1999 .

[30]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Anatomical variability in the cortical representation of first and second language , 1997, Neuroreport.

[32]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  What's right about the neural organization of sign language? A perspective on recent neuroimaging results , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  H. Neville,et al.  Maturational Constraints on Functional Specializations for Language Processing: ERP and Behavioral Evidence in Bilingual Speakers , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.