The Benefits of Buddies: Strategically Pairing Preschoolers with Other-Gender Classmates Promotes Positive Peer Interactions

ABSTRACT Research Findings: We tested whether a universal classroom peer pairing intervention – Buddy Up – would strengthen dyadic peer interactions among preschoolers (N = 140; M age = 56.49 months; 53.6% boys; 77.9% Latinx). The Buddy Up intervention was informed by Intergroup Contact Theory, which specifies the benefits of exposure to heterogeneous others. Teachers buddied children with other-gender peers. We hypothesized positive intervention effects on the quality of dyadic interactions overall and for other-gender interactions specifically. We also considered whether the effects would generalize (secondary transfer effects) to interactions with other-language peers (English and Spanish). Using a quasi-experimental design, we found intervention effects on improved peer interactions overall and for other-gender dyads. There was little evidence of improved interactions for other-language dyads. Same-gender dyads (notably girl-girl dyads) and same-language dyads (notably English-speaking dyads) also responded positively to the intervention. The use of dyadic peer interaction data during preschool represents an innovative feature of this study. Practice or Policy: Buddy Up facilitated positive peer interactions. This is a key building block through which social skills are built and friendships can grow. The findings suggest that early childhood teachers can successfully support students in developing relationships with peers.

[1]  Crystal I. Bryce,et al.  Building integrated peer relationships in preschool classrooms: The potential of buddies , 2021 .

[2]  Zoe Liberman,et al.  Keeping friends in mind: Development of friendship concepts in early childhood , 2020, Social Development.

[3]  K. Olson,et al.  Gender Encoding in Gender Diverse and Gender Conforming Children. , 2020, Child development.

[4]  João R. Daniel,et al.  Changes in preschool children’s social engagement positively predict changes in social competence: A three‐year longitudinal study of portuguese children , 2020, Social Development.

[5]  A. Brian,et al.  SKIPing Together: A Motor Competence Intervention Promotes Gender-Integrated Friendships for Young Children , 2020, Sex Roles.

[6]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Preschool children's math exploration during play with peers , 2019, Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology.

[7]  R. Crisp,et al.  Imagining contact reduces prejudice in preschool children , 2019, Social Development.

[8]  D. Joel,et al.  The Future of Sex and Gender in Psychology: Five Challenges to the Gender Binary , 2019, The American psychologist.

[9]  L. Vezzali,et al.  Secondary transfer effect among children: The role of social dominance orientation and outgroup attitudes , 2018, The British journal of social psychology.

[10]  Cary J. Roseth,et al.  Cooperative Learning in Middle School: A Means to Improve Peer Relations and Reduce Victimization, Bullying, and Related Outcomes , 2018, Journal of educational psychology.

[11]  Linda R. Tropp,et al.  Contact Strategies for Improving Intergroup Relations among Youth , 2017 .

[12]  Nancy L. McElwain,et al.  Child-child similarity on attachment and temperament as predictors of positive interaction during acquaintanceship at age 3. , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[13]  Natalie J. Shook,et al.  The effect of intergroup contact on secondary group attitudes and social dominance orientation , 2016 .

[14]  Linda Lee Peer Relations and Social Adjustment of Latino and Chinese Children in Head Start Classrooms , 2016 .

[15]  Dawn DeLay,et al.  Peer effects on Head Start children's preschool competency. , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[16]  D. Ansari,et al.  Closing the Achievement Gap through Modification of Neurocognitive and Neuroendocrine Function: Results from a Cluster Randomized Controlled Trial of an Innovative Approach to the Education of Children in Kindergarten , 2014, PloS one.

[17]  C. Martin,et al.  Gender-based relationship efficacy: children's self-perceptions in intergroup contexts. , 2014, Child development.

[18]  B. Vaughn,et al.  Dyadic Analyses of Preschool‐aged Children's Friendships: Convergence and Differences between Friendship Classifications from Peer Sociometric Data and Teacher's Reports , 2014 .

[19]  J. Neal,et al.  Classroom Peer Relationships and Behavioral Engagement in Elementary School: The Role of Social Network Equity , 2013, American journal of community psychology.

[20]  E. Pahlke,et al.  Gender-segregated schooling and gender stereotyping , 2013 .

[21]  David R. Schaefer,et al.  The role of sex of peers and gender-typed activities in young children's peer affiliative networks: a longitudinal analysis of selection and influence. , 2012, Child development.

[22]  Katharina Schmid,et al.  Secondary Transfer Effects of Intergroup Contact , 2012 .

[23]  Linda R. Tropp,et al.  Cross-Group Friendships and Intergroup Attitudes , 2011, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[24]  K. Bierman The promise and potential of studying the “invisible hand” of teacher influence on peer relations and student outcomes: A commentary , 2011 .

[25]  T. Farmer,et al.  Revealing the invisible hand: The role of teachers in children's peer experiences , 2011 .

[26]  B. Vaughn,et al.  Are happy children socially successful? Testing a central premise of positive psychology in a sample of preschool children , 2011 .

[27]  Kenneth H. Rubin,et al.  Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups , 2011 .

[28]  Robert W. Faris,et al.  Status Struggles , 2011 .

[29]  Thomas F. Pettigrew,et al.  Secondary transfer effect of contact: Do intergroup contact effects spread to noncontacted outgroups? , 2009 .

[30]  Linda R. Tropp,et al.  A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[31]  Jennifer M. Ferrell,et al.  Interracial contact and racial constancy: A multi-site study of racial intergroup bias in 3-5 year old Anglo-British children , 2005 .

[32]  R. Fabes,et al.  Young children's play qualities in same-, other-, and mixed-sex peer groups. , 2003, Child development.

[33]  J. Dunn,et al.  Old friends, new friends: predictors of children's perspective on their friends at school. , 2002, Child development.

[34]  M. McPherson,et al.  Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks , 2001 .

[35]  J. Olsen,et al.  Peer and Teacher Sociometrics for Preschool Children: Cross-Informant Concordance, Temporal Stability, and Reliability , 2001 .

[36]  B. Vaughn,et al.  Dyadic analyses of friendship in a sample of preschool-age children attending Head Start: correspondence between measures and implications for social competence. , 2001, Child development.

[37]  R. Fabes,et al.  The stability and consequences of young children's same-sex peer interactions. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[38]  A. Eriks-Brophy,et al.  Culturally Based Miscommunication in Classroom Interaction , 1997 .

[39]  G. Ladd,et al.  The Child Behavior Scale : A teacher-report measure of young children's aggressive, withdrawn, and Prosocial behaviors , 1996 .

[40]  R. Bigler The Role of Classification Skill in Moderating Environmental Influences on Children's Gender Stereotyping: A Study of the Functional Use of Gender in the Classroom. , 1995 .

[41]  J Tudor-Hart,et al.  On the nature of prejudice. , 1961, The Eugenics review.

[42]  Linda Heitzman-Powell,et al.  Peer Mediation Interventions to Improve Social and Communication Skills for Children and Youth with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2017 .

[43]  Sabrina Eberhart,et al.  Applied Missing Data Analysis , 2016 .

[44]  K. Rudolph,et al.  A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[45]  B. Vaughn,et al.  Friendship and social competence in a sample of preschool children attending Head Start. , 2000, Developmental psychology.