Centro-parietal EEG potentials index subjective evidence and confidence during perceptual decision making

Recent studies suggest that a centro-parietal positivity (CPP) in the EEG signal tracks the absolute (unsigned) strength of accumulated evidence for choices that require the integration of noisy sensory input. Here, we investigated whether the CPP might also reflect the evidence for decisions based on a quantitative comparison between two sequentially presented stimuli (a signed quantity). We recorded EEG while participants decided whether the latter of two vibrotactile frequencies was higher or lower than the former in six variants of this task (n = 116). To account for biases in sequential comparisons, we applied a behavioral model based on Bayesian inference that estimated subjectively perceived frequency differences. Immediately after the second stimulus, parietal ERPs reflected the signed value of subjectively perceived differences and afterwards their absolute value. Strikingly, the modulation by signed difference was evident in trials without any objective evidence for either choice and correlated with choice-selective premotor beta band amplitudes. Modulations by the absolute strength of subjectively perceived evidence - a direct indicator of task difficulty - exhibited all features of statistical decision confidence. Together, our data suggest that parietal EEG signals first index subjective evidence, and later include a measure of confidence in the context of perceptual decision making.

[1]  R. Romo,et al.  Decoding a Perceptual Decision Process across Cortex , 2010, Neuron.

[2]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Electromagnetic source reconstruction for group studies , 2008, NeuroImage.

[3]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  EEG and MEG Data Analysis in SPM8 , 2011, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[4]  Emilio Salinas,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience: Flutter Discrimination: neural codes, perception, memory and decision making , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[6]  Joshua I. Sanders,et al.  Signatures of a Statistical Computation in the Human Sense of Confidence , 2016, Neuron.

[7]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Bayesian estimation of evoked and induced responses , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[8]  A. Koulakov,et al.  Orbitofrontal Cortex Is Required for Optimal Waiting Based on Decision Confidence , 2014, Neuron.

[9]  Simon Ludwig,et al.  Response-Modality-Specific Encoding of Human Choices in Upper Beta Band Oscillations during Vibrotactile Comparisons , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[10]  Hauke R. Heekeren,et al.  Prior Information Biases Stimulus Representations during Vibrotactile Decision Making , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Rhythmic gain control during supramodal integration of approximate number , 2016, NeuroImage.

[12]  S Sutton,et al.  Event-related potentials in a two-interval forced-choice detection task , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  John W. Morley,et al.  Prior and Present Evidence: How Prior Experience Interacts with Present Information in a Perceptual Decision Making Task , 2012, PloS one.

[14]  R. Romo,et al.  Temporal Evolution of a Decision-Making Process in Medial Premotor Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[15]  Anne E. Urai,et al.  Pupil-linked arousal is driven by decision uncertainty and alters serial choice bias , 2017, Nature Communications.

[16]  Å. Hellström Comparison is not just subtraction: Effects of time- and space-order on subjective stimulus difference , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  Y. Loewenstein,et al.  Bayesian Inference Underlies the Contraction Bias in Delayed Comparison Tasks , 2011, PloS one.

[18]  Simon Ludwig,et al.  Oscillatory EEG signatures of postponed somatosensory decisions , 2018, Human brain mapping.

[19]  Gaëtan Sanchez,et al.  Real-time electrophysiology in cognitive neuroscience : towards adaptive paradigms to study perceptual learning and decision making in humans , 2014 .

[20]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  Jerwen Jou,et al.  Contraction bias in memorial quantifying judgment: does it come from a stable compressed memory representation or a dynamic adaptation process? , 2004, The American journal of psychology.

[22]  S. Kelly,et al.  The classic P300 encodes a build‐to‐threshold decision variable , 2015, The European journal of neuroscience.

[23]  G. Fechner Elemente der Psychophysik , 1998 .

[24]  Simon P. Kelly,et al.  Abstract and Effector-Selective Decision Signals Exhibit Qualitatively Distinct Dynamics before Delayed Perceptual Reports , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  S. Kelly,et al.  Internal and External Influences on the Rate of Sensory Evidence Accumulation in the Human Brain , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data , 2005, NeuroImage.

[27]  Ryota Kanai,et al.  Predictions Shape Confidence in Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  T. Curran Effects of attention and confidence on the hypothesized ERP correlates of recollection and familiarity , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  P. Sajda,et al.  Human Scalp Potentials Reflect a Mixture of Decision-Related Signals during Perceptual Choices , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Vertex potentials evoked during auditory signal detection: Relation to decision criteria , 1973 .

[31]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[32]  Merav Ahissar,et al.  Contradictory Behavioral Biases Result from the Influence of Past Stimuli on Perception , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[33]  Jan Herding,et al.  Neuronal signatures of a random-dot motion comparison task , 2019, NeuroImage.

[34]  H. Woodrow The effect of practice upon time-order errors in the comparison of temporal intervals. , 1935 .

[35]  Ádám Kepecs,et al.  A mathematical framework for statistical decision confidence , 2015, bioRxiv.

[36]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  The sense of flutter-vibration: comparison of the human capacity with response patterns of mechanoreceptive afferents from the monkey hand. , 1968, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Saskia Haegens,et al.  Beta oscillations reflect supramodal information during perceptual judgment , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Felix Blankenburg,et al.  Oscillatory Correlates of Vibrotactile Frequency Processing in Human Working Memory , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Ake Hellstrom,et al.  The time-order error and its relatives: Mirrors of cognitive processes in comparing. , 1985 .

[40]  R. Romo,et al.  Conversion of sensory signals into perceptual decisions , 2013, Progress in Neurobiology.

[41]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Decision Making as a Window on Cognition , 2013, Neuron.

[42]  R. Romo,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of a Perceptual Decision in Ventral Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[43]  Felix Blankenburg,et al.  Stimulus-dependent EEG activity reflects internal updating of tactile working memory in humans , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  Shozo Tobimatsu,et al.  Steady-state vibration somatosensory evoked potentials: physiological characteristics and tuning function , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[45]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[46]  John-Dylan Haynes,et al.  The Relationship between Perceptual Decision Variables and Confidence in the Human Brain. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[47]  Jan Drugowitsch,et al.  Becoming Confident in the Statistical Nature of Human Confidence Judgments , 2016, Neuron.

[48]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Multiple sparse priors for the M/EEG inverse problem , 2008, NeuroImage.

[50]  Lionel Rigoux,et al.  VBA: A Probabilistic Treatment of Nonlinear Models for Neurobiological and Behavioural Data , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[51]  R. Romo,et al.  Beta oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network reflect somatosensory decision making , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[52]  Patrick Berg,et al.  Artifact Correction of the Ongoing EEG Using Spatial Filters Based on Artifact and Brain Signal Topographies , 2002, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[53]  S. Kelly,et al.  The neural processes underlying perceptual decision making in humans: Recent progress and future directions , 2015, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[54]  S. Kelly,et al.  A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[56]  Jan Herding,et al.  Upper Beta Band Oscillations in Human Premotor Cortex Encode Subjective Choices in a Vibrotactile Comparison Task , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  Marios G. Philiastides,et al.  Neural representations of confidence emerge from the process of decision formation during perceptual choices , 2015, NeuroImage.