Segmental anchoring of pitch movements: Autosegmental association or gestural coordination?

Arvaniti, Ladd and Mennen (1998) reported a phenomenon of ‘segmental anchoring’: the beginning and end of a linguistically significant pitch movement are anchored to specific locations in segmental structure, which means that the slope and duration of the pitch movement vary according to the segmental material with which it is associated. This finding has since been replicated and extended in several languages. One possible analysis is that autosegmental tones corresponding to the beginning and end of the pitch movement show secondary association with points in structure; however, problems with this analysis have led some authors to cast doubt on the ‘hypothesis’ of segmental anchoring. I argue here that segmental anchoring is not a hypothesis expressed in terms of autosegmental phonology, but rather an empirical phonetic finding. The difficulty of describing segmental anchoring as secondary association does not disprove the ‘hypothesis’, but shows the error of using a symbolic phonological device (secondary association) to represent gradient differences of phonetic detail that should be expressed quantitatively. I propose that treating pitch movements as gestures (in the sense of Articulatory Phonology) goes some way to resolving some of the theoretical questions raised by segmental anchoring, but suggest that pitch gestures have a variety of ‘domains’ which are in need of empirical study before we can successfully integrate segmental anchoring into our understanding of speech production.

[1]  I. Lehiste The Timing of Utterances and Linguistic Boundaries , 1972 .

[2]  J. T. Hart,et al.  Integrating different levels of intonation analysis , 1975 .

[3]  Peter F. MacNeilage,et al.  The Production of Speech , 2011, Springer New York.

[4]  Hiroya Fujisaki,et al.  Dynamic Characteristics of Voice Fundamental Frequency in Speech and Singing , 1983 .

[5]  Louis Goldstein,et al.  Towards an articulatory phonology , 1986, Phonology.

[6]  Kim E. A. Silverman,et al.  The timing of prenuclear high accents in English , 1987 .

[7]  J. Pierrehumbert The phonology and phonetics of English intonation , 1987 .

[8]  J. Pierrehumbert,et al.  Japanese Tone Structure , 1988 .

[9]  Bruce P. Hayes Compensatory Lengthening in Moraic Phonology , 1989 .

[10]  John Kingston,et al.  Papers in Laboratory Phonology: Index of names , 1990 .

[11]  J S Perkell,et al.  Temporal measures of anticipatory labial coarticulation for the vowel/u/: within- and cross-subject variability. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  J Caspers,et al.  Effects of Time Pressure on the Phonetic Realization of the Dutch Accent-Lending Pitch Rise and Fall , 1993, Phonetica.

[13]  Julia Hirschberg,et al.  Tonal alignment patterns in Spanish , 1995 .

[14]  D. Robert Ladd,et al.  Tonal Alignment and the Representation of Accentual Targets , 1995 .

[15]  Markus Hiller,et al.  Diphthong dynamics in Swabian , 1996 .

[16]  Elizabeth C. Zsiga,et al.  Features, Gestures, and Igbo Vowels: An Approach to the Phonology-Phonetics Interface , 1997 .

[17]  D. Ladd,et al.  Stability of tonal alignment: the case of Greek prenuclear accents , 1998 .

[18]  Y Xu,et al.  Consistency of Tone-Syllable Alignment across Different Syllable Structures and Speaking Rates , 1998, Phonetica.

[19]  Yi Xu,et al.  Effects of tone and focus on the formation and alignment of f0contours , 1999 .

[20]  James M Scobbie,et al.  Morphemes, Phonetics and Lexical Items: The Case of the Scottish Vowel Length Rule. , 1999 .

[21]  D. Ladd,et al.  Constant "segmental anchoring" of F0 movements under changes in speech rate. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  Taehong Cho,et al.  Variation and universals in VOT: evidence from 18 languages , 1999 .

[23]  Stefanie Shattuck-Hufnagel,et al.  Word-boundary-related duration patterns in English , 2000, J. Phonetics.

[24]  P Taylor,et al.  Analysis and synthesis of intonation using the Tilt model. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[25]  Amalia Arvaniti,et al.  On the place of phrase accents in intonational phonology , 2000, Phonology.

[26]  D. Ladd,et al.  Phonological conditioning of peak alignment in rising pitch accents in Dutch. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  Emily Q. Wang,et al.  Pitch targets and their realization: Evidence from Mandarin Chinese , 2001, Speech Commun..

[28]  Timothy L. Face,et al.  Intonational marking of contrastive focus in Madrid Spanish , 2002 .

[29]  J. Masefield,et al.  ‘ ‘ Sagging transitions ’ ’ between high pitch accents in English : experimental evidence , 2003 .

[30]  Takeshi Ishihar,et al.  A phonological effect on tonal alig , 2003 .

[31]  D. Robert Ladd,et al.  "Sagging transitions" between high pitch accents in English: experimental evidence , 2003, J. Phonetics.

[32]  D. Robert Ladd,et al.  On the phonetics and phonology of "segmental anchoring" of F0: evidence from German , 2004, J. Phonetics.

[33]  D. R. Ladd,et al.  Segmental anchoring of pitch movements: autosegmental phonology or speech production? , 2004 .

[34]  Astrid Schepman,et al.  Alignment of “Phrase Accent” Lows in Dutch Falling Rising Questions: Theoretical and Methodological Implications , 2005, Language and speech.

[35]  María Dolores Ramírez Verdugo Timothy L. Face (2002): Intonational Marking of Contrastive Focus in Madrid Spanish, Lincom Europa, Muenchen , 2005 .

[36]  D. Robert Ladd,et al.  Alignment of L and H in bitonal pitch accents: testing two hypotheses , 2005, J. Phonetics.

[37]  P. Prieto,et al.  Pitch Accent Alignment in Romance: Primary and Secondary Associations with Metrical Structure , 2005, Language and speech.

[38]  T. Ishihara,et al.  Tonal alignment in Tokyo Japanese , 2006 .

[39]  Robin J. Lickley,et al.  Effects of vowel length and "right context" on the alignment of Dutch nuclear accents , 2006, J. Phonetics.

[40]  Amalia Arvaniti,et al.  Dialectal variation in the rising accents of American English , 2007 .