Differential Activation of Right Superior Parietal Cortex and Intraparietal Sulcus by Spatial and Nonspatial Attention

Neuropsychological and functional neuroimaging studies have implicated the right posterior parietal cortex (PPC) in human spatial attention. We tested the hypothesis that this area is also involved in nonspatial aspects of attention and working memory using positron emission tomography in healthy volunteers. In an initial experiment, digits were presented in pseudo-random spatial locations, and subjects attended either to locations or digits in order to detect single targets (attention condition) or to sequences of stimuli (working memory (WM) condition). Right superior parietal cortex (BA7) and intraparietal sulcus (IPS) were active during both spatial (locations) and nonspatial (digits) tasks compared to rest, although more so for the former. Additionally, right PPC was activated to an even greater extent during tests of WM than of attention, especially for tests of spatial WM. There were no differences in activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the spatial versus nonspatial versions of the task, contrary to many previous studies. A follow-up experiment which presented abstract objects in a fixed, central location confirmed that right IPS was active during tests of nonspatial attention and also that this activation is not due to incidental spatial representation of digit stimuli. However, BA7 was not activated by this nonspatial, nondigit attentional task. Overall, these data suggest first that right IPS is recruited for both nonspatial and spatial attention and WM. Second, right BA7 is recruited specifically for spatial (both direct and indirect) forms of attentional processing. Finally, PPC activations in spatial WM tasks are likely to be due to a combination of spatial perception, attention, and WM, rather than to any of these individually.

[1]  Don McNicol,et al.  A Primer of Signal Detection Theory , 1976 .

[2]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  M. Mesulam A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.

[4]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[5]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .

[6]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  R. Andersen Visual and eye movement functions of the posterior parietal cortex. , 1989, Annual review of neuroscience.

[8]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Relationship between Global and Local Changes in PET Scans , 1990, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[9]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism Comparing Functional (pet) Images: the Assessment of Significant Change , 2022 .

[10]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  S. Dehaene Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.

[12]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. , 1993, Science.

[13]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[14]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  Alan D. Baddeley,et al.  Working memory or working attention , 1993 .

[16]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET , 1993, Nature.

[18]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Attention : selection, awareness, and control : a tribute to Donald Broadbent , 1996 .

[19]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory , 1993, Nature.

[20]  M. Corbetta,et al.  PET studies of parietal involvement in spatial attention: comparison of different task types. , 1994, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[21]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[22]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Superior Parietal Cortex Activation During Spatial Attention Shifts and Visual Feature Conjunction , 1995, Science.

[23]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Spatial versus Object Working Memory: PET Investigations , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[25]  J Jonides,et al.  Human Rehearsal Processes and the Frontal Lobes: PET Evidence , 1995, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[26]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Activation of human prefrontal cortex during spatial and nonspatial working memory tasks measured by functional MRI. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  T. Goldberg,et al.  Isolating the Mnemonic Component in Spatial Delayed Response: A Controlled PET15O-Labeled Water Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Study in Normal Humans , 1996, NeuroImage.

[28]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  L. McEvoy,et al.  High resolution evoked potential imaging of the cortical dynamics of human working memory. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[30]  G. Orban,et al.  The influence of stimulus location on the brain activation pattern in detection and orientation discrimination. A PET study of visual attention. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  J. Jonides,et al.  Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  PET Evidence for an Amodal Verbal Working Memory System , 1996, NeuroImage.

[34]  C. Frith,et al.  A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: a PET study of sustained attention and working memory , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  M. Petrides,et al.  Specialized systems for the processing of mnemonic information within the primate frontal cortex. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[36]  R. Andersen,et al.  Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[37]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Cognitive Conjunction: A New Approach to Brain Activation Experiments , 1997, NeuroImage.

[38]  A. Owen The Functional Organization of Working Memory Processes Within Human Lateral Frontal Cortex: The Contribution of Functional Neuroimaging , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[39]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  R. Dolan,et al.  The Neural Correlates of the Noradrenergic Modulation of Human Attention, Arousal and Learning , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[41]  C. Frith,et al.  Monitoring for target objects: activation of right frontal and parietal cortices with increasing time on task , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  A. Nobre,et al.  Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.