Kinematic analysis of reaching in the cat

The present study examines the kinematic features of forelimb movements made by cats reaching for food in horizontal target wells located at different heights and distances. Wrist paths consisted of two relatively straight segments joined at a “via-point” in front of the aperture of the food well. In the initial lift phase, the paw was raised to the via-point in front of the target. In the second, or thrust phase, the paw was directed forward into the food well. During the lift, the paw was moved toward the target primarily by elbow flexion, accompanied by a sequence of biphasic shoulder and wrist movements. Thrust was accomplished primarily by shoulder flexion while the wrist and the paw were maintained at near-constant angles. The animals varied the height of the reach primarily by varying elbow flexion with proportional changes in elbow angular velocity and angular acceleration and with corresponding variations in wrist speed. Thus, cats reached for targets at different heights by scaling a common kinematic profile. Over a relatively large range of target heights, animals maintained movement duration constant, according to a simple “pulse-height” control strategy (isochronous scaling). For reaches to a given target height, animals compensated for variability in peak acceleration by variations in movement time. We examined the coordination between the shoulder and the wrist with the elbow. Early during the lift, peak shoulder extensor and peak elbow flexor accelerations were synchronized. Late during the lift phase, wrist extensor acceleration was found to occur during the period of elbow flexor deceleration. We hypothesize that these linkages could, in part, be due to passive mechanical interactions. To determine how the angular trajectories of the different joints were organized in relation to target location, we plotted joint kinematic changes directly on the wrist and MCP joint paths. These plots revealed that for all target heights and movement speeds, wrist extensor deceleration occurred at approximately the same spatial location with respect to the target. This analysis also demonstrated that the second phase of MCP flexion occurred when the paw was below the lower lip of the food well, while the subsequent extension occurred after the tip cleared this obstacle. During thrust, wrist and MCP angles were maintained, reflecting the need to align the paw within the food well. Our findings suggest that cats plan the reaching phase of prehension as a sequence of discrete movement segments, each serving a particular goal in the task, rather than as an single unit. The presence of straight trajectory segments suggests that cats, like humans, plan movements of their paw in extrinsic rather than joint space.

[1]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[3]  Carole Levin,et al.  Physical Examination of the Spine and Extremities , 1977 .

[4]  D. Vicario,et al.  The control of rapid limb movement in the cat II. Scaling of isometric force adjustments , 1978, Experimental brain research.

[5]  E. Sybirska,et al.  Effects of pyramidal lesions on forelimb movements in the cat. , 1980, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[6]  C. K. Yuen,et al.  Digital Filters , 1979, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[7]  P. Viviani,et al.  Trajectory determines movement dynamics , 1982, Neuroscience.

[8]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Human arm trajectory formation. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  P. Viviani,et al.  The relation between linear extent and velocity in drawing movements , 1983, Neuroscience.

[10]  H. Yumiya,et al.  Sensory motor processing of target movements in motor cortex. , 1983, Advances in neurology.

[11]  M. Jeannerod The timing of natural prehension movements. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[12]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  P Viviani,et al.  Segmentation and coupling in complex movements. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  Ronald F. Zernicke,et al.  Predictions for neural control based on limb dynamics , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[16]  A. Wing,et al.  Relation between velocity and curvature in movement: equivalence and divergence between a power law and a minimum-jerk model. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  N Hogan,et al.  Planning and execution of multijoint movements. , 1988, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[18]  G. Gottlieb,et al.  Strategies for the control of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom , 1989, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[19]  K M Heilman,et al.  Three-dimensional computergraphic analysis of apraxia. Neural representations of learned movement. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  C Ghez,et al.  Roles of proprioceptive input in the programming of arm trajectories. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[21]  Differential spinal projections of subregions in the forelimb area of the motor cortex in the cat , 1993, Neuroscience Letters.

[22]  C. Ghez,et al.  Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  R. Johansson,et al.  Somatosensory control of precision grip during unpredictable pulling loads , 1992, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  R. Johansson,et al.  Somatosensory control of precision grip during unpredictable pulling loads , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  C. Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 1987, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  C. Ghez,et al.  Differential impairments in reaching and grasping produced by local inactivation within the forelimb representation of the motor cortex in the cat , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  P. Morasso Spatial control of arm movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  S. Cooper,et al.  Differential effects of local inactivation within motor cortex and red nucleus on performance of an elbow task in the cat , 1993, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  J. Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  John M. Hollerbach,et al.  Dynamic interactions between limb segments during planar arm movement , 1982, Biological Cybernetics.

[31]  H. Freund,et al.  The relationship between speed and amplitude of the fastest voluntary contractions of human arm muscles , 1978, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  E. Sybirska,et al.  Integration in descending motor pathways controlling the forelimb in the cat , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Y. Pai,et al.  The organization of torque and EMG activity during bilateral handle pulls by standing humans , 1990, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  C. Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 1987, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  B. Alstermark,et al.  Characteristics of target-reaching in cats , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.