The Use of Definite References Signals Declarative Memory

Language function in patients with impaired declarative memory presents a compelling opportunity to investigate the inter-dependence of memory and language in referential communication. We examined amnesic patients’ use of definite references during a referential communication task. Discursively, definite references can be used to mark a referent as situationally unique (e.g., “the game,” as in the case of a recently publicized game) or as shared information (e.g., “the game,” as in one discussed previously). We found that despite showing normal collaborative learning after repeated referring—as indexed by consistent and increasingly efficient descriptive labels for previously unfamiliar tangram figures—amnesic patients did not consistently use definite references in referring to those figures. The use of definite references seems to be critically dependent on declarative memory, and the engagement of such memory is signaled by language.

[1]  Neal J Cohen,et al.  Hippocampal amnesia disrupts verbal play and the creative use of language in social interaction , 2009, Aphasiology.

[2]  N. Cohen,et al.  Successful life outcome and management of real-world memory demands despite profound anterograde amnesia , 2008, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[3]  Ron Dumont,et al.  Wechsler Memory Scale–Third Edition , 2008 .

[4]  John O. Willis,et al.  Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition , 2008 .

[5]  Neal J. Cohen,et al.  Collaborative discourse facilitates efficient communication and new learning in amnesia , 2008, Brain and Language.

[6]  Julie A. Hengst,et al.  Hippocampal amnesia disrupts the flexible use of procedural discourse in social interaction , 2008, Aphasiology.

[7]  Neal J Cohen,et al.  Talking across time: Using reported speech as a communicative resource in amnesia , 2007, Aphasiology.

[8]  A. Agha Language and Social Relations: References , 2006 .

[9]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Correlations between Regional Brain Volumes and Memory Performance in Anoxia , 2006, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[10]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  The scope of preserved procedural memory in amnesia. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  M. Pickering,et al.  Why is conversation so easy? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  Julie A. Hengst,et al.  Collaborative referencing between individuals with aphasia and routine communication partners. , 2003, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[13]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The Boston naming test , 2001 .

[14]  E. Kandel,et al.  Cognitive Neuroscience and the Study of Memory , 1998, Neuron.

[15]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  J. Hodges Memory, Amnesia and the Hippocampal System , 1995 .

[17]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Coordinating beliefs in conversation , 1992 .

[18]  Neal J Cohen,et al.  Development of shared information in communication despite hippocampal amnesia , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  Philip R. Cohen,et al.  Referring as a Collaborative Process , 2003 .

[20]  N. Cohen From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection Memory Systems of the Brain. Oxford Psychology Series, Volume 35. , 2001 .

[21]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection , 2001 .

[22]  Donald G. MacKay,et al.  H.M.'s Language Production Deficits: Implications for Relations between Memory, Semantic Binding, and the Hippocampal System ☆ ☆☆ ★ , 1998 .

[23]  D. Tranel Neurobiological foundations of human memory , 1995 .

[24]  H. H. Clark Arenas of language use , 1993 .